Approved For Release bQ3106124: CIATRDR79-e6999AO00300060005-1 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, VOL. 64, NO. 3, MARCH 1976 A Perceptual Channel for Information Transfer over Kilometer Distances: Historical Perspective and Recent Research HAROLD E. PUTHOFF, MEMBER, IEEE, AND R1J'N,1;F.1T TARG, SENIOR MEMBER, IEEE Abstract-For more than 100 years, scientists have attempted to determine the truth or falsity of claims for the existence of a perceptual channel whereby certain individuals are able to perceive and describe remote data not presented to any known sense. This paper presents an outtine of the history of scientific inquiry into such so-called paranor- mal perception and surveys the current state of the art in parapsycho- logical research in the United States and abroad. The nature of this perceptual channel is examined in a series of experiments carried out in the Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory of Stanford Research Institute. The perceptual modality most extensively investigated is the ability of both experienced subjects and inexperienced volunteers to view, by innate mental processes, remote geographical or technical targets including buildings, roads, and laboratory apparatus. The ae- cumulated data indicate that the phenomenon is not a sensitive func- tion of distance, and Faraday cage shielding does not in any apparent way degrade the quality and accuracy of perception. On the basis of this research, some areas of physics are suggested from which a descrip- tion or explanation of the phenomenon could be forthcoming. 1. INTRODUCTION "IT IS THE PROVINCE of natural science to investigate nature, impartially and without prejudice" [I]. Nowhere in scientific inquiry has this dictum met as great a chal- lenge as in the area of so-called extrasensory perception (ESP), the detection of remote stimuli not mediated by the usual sensory processes. Such phenomena, although under scientific consideration for over a century, have historically been fraught with unreliability and controversy, and validation of the phe- nomena by accepted scientific methodology has been slow in coming. Even so, a recent survey conducted by the British publication New Scientist revealed that 67 percent of nearly 1500 responding readers (the majority of whom are working sctentists and technologists) considered ESP to be an estab- lished fact or a likely possibility, and 88 percent held the investigation of ESP to be a legitimate scientific undertaking [,I I - A review of the literature reveals that although experiments by reputable researchers yielding positive results were begun oNer a century ago (e.g., Sir William Crookes' study of D. D. Home, 1860's) [31, many consider the study of these phe- nomena as only recently emerging from the realm of quasi- science. One reason for this is that, despite experimental results, no satisfactory theoretical construct had been advanced to correlate data or to predict new experimental outcomes. Consequently, the area in question remained for a long time in the recipe stage reminiscent of electrodynamics before the Manuscript received July 25, 1975; revised November 7, 1975. The submission of this paper was encouraged after review of an advance proposal. This work was supported by the Foundation for Parasensory Investigation and the Parapsychology Foundation, New York, NY; the Institute of Noetic Sciences, Palo Alto, CA; and the National Aero- nautics and Space Administration, under Contract NAS 7-100. The authors are with the Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory, Stanford Research Institute, Menlo Park, CA 94025. 329 unification brought about by the work of Ampere, Faraday, and Maxwell. Since the early work, however, we have seen the development of information theory, quantum theory, and neuro physiological research, and these disciplines provide powerful conceptual tools that appear to bear directly on the issue. In fact, several physicists (Section V) are now of the opinion that these phenomena are not at all inconsistent with the framework of modern physics: the often-held view that observations of this type are a priori incompatible with known laws is erroneous in that such a concept is based on the naive realism prevalent before the development of quantum theory. In the emerging view, it is accepted that research in this area can be conducted so as to uncover not just a catalog of inter- esting events, but rather patterns of cause-effect relationships of the type that lend themselves to analysis and hypothesis in the forms with which we are familiar in the physical sciences. One hypothesis is that information transfer under conditions of sensory shielding is mediated by extremely low-frequency (ELF) electromagnetic waves, a proposal that does not seem to be ruled out by any obvious physical or biological facts. Further, the development of information theory makes it possible to characterize and quantify the performance of a communications channel regardless of the underlying mechanism. For the past three years, we have had a program in the Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory of the Stan- ford Research Institute (SRI) to investigate those facets of human perception that appear to fall outside the range of well- understood perceptual/processing capabilities. Of particular interest is a human in format ion-a cce ssing capability that we call "remote viewing." This phenomenon pertains to the ability of certain individuals to access and describe, by means of mental processes, information sources blocked from ordi- nary perception, and generally accepted as secure against such access. In particular, the phenomenon we have investigated most extensively is the ability of a subject to view remote geograph- ical locations up to several thousand kilometers distant from his physical location (given only a known person on whom to target). 1 We have carried out more than fifty experiments under controlled laboratory conditions with several individuals whose remote perceptual abilities have been developed suf- ficiently to allow them at times to describe correctly- often in great detail -ge ogra phi cal or technical material such as build- ings, roads, laboratory apparatus, and the like. As observed in the laboratory, the basic phenomenon appears to cover a range of subjective experiences variously referred to 'Our initial work in this area was reported in Nature 14 1, and re- printed in the IEEE Commun. Soc. Newsletter, vol. 13, Jan. 1975 Copyright @ 1976 by The Institute of Electrical and Liectronics Engineers, Inc. Printed in U.S.A. Annals No. 003PR004 Approved For Release 2003/06/24: CIA-RDP79,nO999AO00300060005-1 Approved For Release 2003/06/24: CIA-RDP79-0090 @_,*000300060005-1 330 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 4 "'1 I'7J /.'.2 S - 1-2 0 d'- Fig. 1. Airport in San Andres, Colombia, used as remote-viewing target, along with sketch produced by subject in California. in the literature as autoscopy (in the medical literature); exteri- orization or disassociation (psychological literature); simple clairvoyance, traveling clairvoyance, or out-of-body experience (parapsychological literature); or astral projection (occult liter- ature). We choose the term "remote viewing" as a neutral descriptive term free from prior associations and bias as to mechanisms. The development at SRI of a successful experimental pro- cedure to elicit this capability has evolved to the point where persons such as visiting government scientists and contract monitors, with no previous exposure to such concepts, have learned to perform well; and subjects who have trained over a one-year period have performed excellently under a variety of experimental conditions. Our accumulated data thus indicate that both specially selected and unselected persons can be assisted in developing remote perceptual abilities up to a level of useful information transfer. In experiments of this type, we have three principal findings. First, we have established that it is possible to obtain signifi- cant amounts of accurate descriptive information about remote locations. Second, an increase in the distance from a few meters up to 4000 km separating the subject from the scene to be perceived does not in any apparent way degrade the quality or accuracy of perception. Finally, the use of Faraday cage electrical shielding does not prevent high-quality descrip- tions from being obtained. To build a coherent theory for the explanation of these phenomena, it is necessary to have a clear understanding of what constitutes the phenomena. In this paper, we first briefly summarize previous efforts in this field in Section 11. We then present in Sections III and IV the results of a series of more Approved For Release ZOP3/06/24 than fifty experiments with nine subjects carried out in our own laboratory, which represent a sufficiently stable data base to permit testing of various hypotheses concerning the func- tioning of this channel. Finally, in Section V, we indicate those areas of physics and information theory that appear to be relevant to an understanding of certain aspects of the phenomena. First, however, we present an illustrative example generated in an early pilot experiment. As will be clear from our later discussion, this is not a "best-ever" example, but rather a typical sample of the level of proficiency that can be reached and that we have come to expect in our research. Three subjects participated in a long-distance experiment focusing on a series of targets in Costa Rica. These subjects said they had never been to Costa Rica. In this experiment, one of the experimenters (Dr. Puthoff) spent ten days traveling through Costa Rica on a combination business/ pleasure trip. This information was all that was known to the subjects about the traveler's itinerary. The experiment called for Dr. Puthoff to keep a detailed record of his location and activities, includ- ing photographs of each of seven target days at 1330 PDT. A total of twelve daily descriptions were collected before the traveler's return: six responses from one subject, five from another, and one from a third. The third subject who submitted the single response supplied a drawing for a day in the middle of the series. (The subject's response, together with the photographs taken at the site, are shown in Fig. 1). Although Costa Rica is a mountainous country, the subject unexpectedly perceived the traveler at a beach and ocean setting. With some misgiving, he described an airport on a sandy beach and an airstrip with the ocean at the CIA-RDP79-00999AO00300060005-1 Approved For Release 2643/06/24: CIA-RDP79-00§99AO00300060005-1 Pt THOFF AND TARG: PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER 331 enJ (correct). An airport building also was drawn, and shown to have a large rectangular overhang (correct). The traveler haJ taken an unplanned one-day side trip to an offshore island and at the time of the experiment had just disembarked from a plane at a small island airport as described by the subject 4000 km away. The sole discrepancy was that the subject's drawing showed a Quonset-hut type of building in place of the rectangular structure. The above description was chosen as an example to illustrate a major point observed a number of times throughout the program to be described. Contrary to what may be expected, a subject's description does not necessarily portray what may reasonably be expected to be correct (an educated or "safe" guess), but often runs counter even to the subject's own expectations. We wish to stress again that a result such as the above is not uriusual. The remaining submissions in this experiment pro- vitled further examples of excellent correspondences between target and response. (A target period of poolside relaxation was identified; a drive through a tropical forest at the base of a @runcated volcano was described as a drive through a jungle below a large bare table mountain; a hotel-room target descrip- tion, including such details as rug color, was correct; and so or,.) So as to determine whether such matches were simply fortuitous-that is, could reasonably be expected on the basis of chance alone-Dr. Puthoff was asked after he had returned to blind match the twelve descriptions to his seven target locations. On the basis of this conservative evaluation proce- dure, which vastly underestimates the statistical significance of the individual descriptions, five correct matches were ob- tained. This number of matches is significant at p = 0.02 by exact binomial calculation.2 The observation of such unexpectedly high-quality descrip- tions early in our program led to a large-scale study of the phenomenon at SRI under secure double-blind conditions (i.e., target unknown to experimenters as well as subjects), with independent random target selection and blind judging. The results, presented in Sections III and IV, provide strong evi- &nce for the robustness of this phenomenon whereby a human perceptual modality of extreme sensitivity can detect complex remote stimuli. 11. BACKGROUND Although we are approaching the study of these phenomena as physicists, it is not yet possible to separate ourselves entirely from the language of the nineteenth century when the labora- tory study of the paranormal was begun. Consequently, we continue to use terms such as "paranormal," "telepathy," and the like. However, we intend only to indicate a process of information transfer under conditions generally accepted as secure against such transfer and with no prejudice or occult assumptions as to the mechanisms involved. As in any other scientific pursuit, the purpose is to collect the observables that result from experiments and to try to determine the functional relationships between these observables and the laws of physics as they are currently understood. 2The probability of a correct daily match by chance for any given transcript is p = -L. Therefore, the probability of at least five correct 7 matches by chance out of twelve tries can be calculated from 1" 12-1 .) P 0.02. i! 12 - i)! 7)1(7 j=5 Organized research into so-called psychic functioning began roughly in the time of J. J. Thomson, Sir Oliver Lodge, and Sir William Crookes, all of whom took part in the founding of the Society for Psychical Research (SPR) in 1882 in England. Crookes, for example, carried out his principal investigations with D. D, Home, a Scotsman who grew up in America and returned to England in 185 5 131 . According to the notebooks and published reports of Crookes, Home had demonstrated the ability to cause objects to move without touching them. We should note in passing that, Home, unlike most subjects, worked only in the light and spoke out in the strongest pos- sible terms against the darkened seance rooms popular at the time [ 5 ]. Sir William Crookes was a pioneer in the study of electrical discharge in gases and in the development of vacuum tubes, some types of which still bear his name. Although everything Crookes said about electron beams and plasmas was accepted, nothing he said about the achievements of D. D. Home ever achieved that status. Many of his colleagues, who had not observed the experiments with Home, stated publicly that they thought Crookes had been deceived, to which Crookes angrily responded: Will not my critics give me credit for some amount of common sense? Do they not imagine that the obvious precautions, which occur to them as soon as they sit down to pick holes in my experiments, have occurred to me also in the course of my pro- longed and patient investigation? The answer to this, as to all other objections is, prove it to be an error, by showing where the error lies, or if a trick, by showing how the trick is per- formed. Try the experiment fully and fairly. If then fraud be found, expose it; if it be a truth, proclaim it. This is the only scientific procedure, and it is that I propose steadily to pursue [31- In the United States, scientific interest in the paranormal was centered in the universities. In 1912, John Coover [61 was established in the endowed Chair of Psychical Research at Stanford University. In the 1920's, Harvard University set up research programs with George Estabrooks and L. T. Troland [71, [8]. It was in this framework that, in 1930, William McDougall invited Dr. J. B. Rhine and Dr. Louisa Rhine to join the Psychology Department at Duke University [9]. For more than 3 0 years, significant work was carried out at Rhine's Duke University Laboratory. To examine the existence of paranormal perception, he used the now-famous ESP cards containing a boldly printed picture of a star, cross, square, circle, or wavy lines. Subjects were asked to name the order of these cards in a freshly shuffled deck of twenty-five such cards. To test for telepathy, an experimenter would look at the cards one at a time, and a subject suitably separated from the sender would attempt to determine which card was being viewed. Dr. J. B. Rhine together with Dr. J. G. Pratt carried out thousands of experiments of this type under widely varying conditions [101 . The statistical results from these experiments indicated that some individuals did indeed possess a paranor- mal perceptual ability in that it was possible to obtain an arbitrarily high degree of improbability by continued testing of a gifted subject. The work of Rhine has been challenged on many grounds, however, including accusations of improper handling of statis- tics, error, and fraud. With regard to the statistics, the general consensus of statisticians today is that if fault is to be found in Rhine's work, it would have to be on other than statistical grounds [I I]. With regard to the accusations of fraud, the " , r", --An Tv 0999AO00300060005-1 Approved For Release 2003/06/24 :.CIA-f:Z @@, , @; , @- I ,.@ -4,. k a 3: 2 Approved For Release 200/06/24 : CIA-RDP79-00.99$&QP,"QP09901§ri.I IFTE, MARCH 1976 frost celebrated case of criticism of Rhine's work, that of G R. Price [121, ended 17 years after it began when the at cusation of fraud was retracted by its author in an article ej titled "Apology to Rhine and Soal," published in the same jc.urnal in which it was first put forward 1131. It should also bA noted that parapsychological researchers themselves re- ct ntly exposed fraud in their own laboratory when they ei countered it [ 14 1. 4kt the end of the 1940's, Prof. S. G. Soal, an English mathe- m itician working with the SPR, had carried out hundreds of card guessing experiments involving tens of thousands of calls [IS I. Many of these experiments were carried out over ex- te ided distances. One of the most notable experiments was cc nducted with Mrs. Gloria Stewart between London and Antwerp. This experiment gave results whose probability of occurring by chance were less than 10-8. Witli the publication of Modern Experiments in Telepathy by Soal and Bateman (b )th of whom were statisticians), it appeared that card guess- ini, experiments produced significant results, on the average. 3 "he most severe criticism of all this work, a criticism diffi- cu t to defend against in principle, is that leveled by the well- known British parapsychological critic C. E. M. Hansel f 171, wl o began his examination of the ESP hypothesis with the stated assumption, "In view of the a priori arguments against it we know in advance that telepathy, etc., cannot occur." Th@refore, based on the "a priori unlikelihood" of ESP, Hansel's examination of the literature centered primarily on tht possibility of fraud, by subjects or investigators. He reviewed in depth four experiments which he regarded as providing the best evidence of ESP: the Pearce-Pratt distance series [181; the Pratt-Woodruff [19] series, both conducted at Duke; and Soal's work with Mrs. Stewart and Basil Shackle- toi [ 15 1, as well as a more recent series by Soal and Bowden 12t I]. Hansel showed, in each ease, how fraud could have been committed (by the experimenters in the Pratt-Woodruff and SoA-Bateman series, or by the subjects in the PeaTce-Pratt anc Soal-Bowden experiments). He gave no direct evidence that fraud was committed in these experiments, but said, "If the result could have arisen through a trick, the experiment must be considered unsatisfactory proof of ESP, whether or no,, it is finally decided that such a trick was in fact used " [ 17, P. 181. As discussed by Honorton in a review of the field [ 2 11, Hansel's conclusion after 241 pages of careful scrutiny therefore was that these experiments were not "fraud-proof" and therefore in principle could not serve as conclusive proof of 1"ISP. Even among the supporters of ESP research and its results, the -e remained the consistent problem that many successful subjects eventually lost their ability and their scores gradually dritted toward chance results. This decline effect in no way era,sed their previous astronomical success; but it was a disap- poiatment since if paranormal perception is a natural ability, one would like to see subjects improving with practice rather thari getting worse. One of the first successful attempts to overcome the decline effect was in Czechoslovakia in the work of Dr. Milan RyzI, a chemist with the Institute of Biology of the Czechoslovakian Academy of Science and also an amateur hypnotist 1221. Through the use of hypnosis, together with feedback and reinforcement, he developed several outstanding subjects, one of whom, Pavel Stepanek, has worked with experimenters around the world for more than 10 years. Ryzl's pioneering work came as an answer to the questions raised by the 1956 CIBA Foundation conference on extra- sensory perception. The CIBA Chemical Company has annual meetings on topics of biological and chemical interest, and that same year they assembled several prominent parapsy- chologists to have a state-of-the-art conference on ESP [ 23 1 . The conference concluded that little progress would be made in parapsychology research until a repeatable experiment could be found; namely, an experiment that different experi- menters could repeat at will and that would reliabl@ yield a statistically significant result. Ryzl had by 1962 accomplished that goal. His primary con- tribution was a decision to interact with the subject as a per- son, to try to build up his confidence and ability. His protocol depended on "working with" rather than "running" his sub- jects. RyzI's star subject, Pavel Stepanek, has produced highly significant results with many contemporary researchers [ 241 - [291. In these experiments, he was able to tell with 60-percent reliability whether a hidden card was green side or white side up, yielding statistics of a million to one with only a thousand trials. As significant as such results are statistically, the information channel is imperfect, containing noise along with the signal. When considering how best to use such a channel, one is led to the communication theory concept of the introduction of' redundancy as a means of coding a message to combat the effects of a noisy channel [301. A prototype experiment by Ryz1 using such techniques has proved to be successful. RyzI had an assistant select randomly five groups of three digits each. These 15 digits were then encoded into binary form and translated into a sequence of green and white cards in seated envelopes. By means of repeated calling and an elaborate majority vote protocol, RyzI was able after 19 350 calls by Stepanek (averaging 9 s per call) to correctly identify all 15 numbers, a result significant at p = 10-15 The hit rate for individual calls was 61.9 percent, 1] 978 hits, and 7372 misses [311. Note Added in Proof- It has been brought to our attention that a similar procedure was recently used to transmit without error the word "peace" in International Morse Code (J. C. Carpenter, "Toward the effective utilization of enhanced weak-signal ESP effects," presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, New York, NY, Jan. 27, 1975). The characteristics of such a channel can be specified in accordance with the precepts of communication theory. The bit rate associated with the information channel is calculated from [301 R = H(x) - Hy W where H(x) is the uncertainty of the source message containing symbols with a priori probability pi: 2 H(X) Pi 1092 Pi (2) and Hy GO is the conditional entropy based on the a posteriori probabilities that a received signal was actually transmitted: 311'ecently, some of the early Soal experiments have been criticized 16 1. However, his long-distance experiments cited here were judged in a double-blind fashion of the type that escaped the criticism of the earl @ experiments. 2 Hy W P U, Ji) 1092 Pi G) (3) -00999AO00300060005-1 Approved For Release 2003106124: CIA-RDP7J PUHOFF AND TARcAppfpm",EiPFERWftve.*RWOAa4it)PW~QPPW0999AO00300060005-1 333 1 Fcr Stepanek's run, with pi = I , pi (j) = 0.619, and an average tinie of 9 s per choice, we have a source uncertainty H(x) = I bit and a calculated bit rate or R - 0.041 bit/symbol RIT - 0.0046 bit/s. Similar behavior modification types of experiments have beeii carried out in recent times by 1. M. Kogan, Chairman of the Bioinformation Section of the Moscow Board of the Popov Society. He is a Soviet engineer who, until 1961), published extensively on the theory of telepathic communicatior 1371 - [40 1. He was concerned with three principal kinds of expe6 ments: mental suggestion without hypnosis over short dis- tances, in which the percipient attempts to identify an object, mental awakening over short distances, in which a subject is awakened from a hypnotic sleep at the "beamed" suggestion from the hypnotist; and long-range (intercity) telepathic com- munication. Kogan's main interest has been to quantify tht, channel capacity of the paranormal channel. fie finds that the bit rate decreases from 0.1 bit/s for laboratory Lxperinients to 0.005 bit/s for his 1000-km intercity experiments. In the USSR, serious consideration is given to the hypothesis that telepathy is mediated by extremely low-freqlAency (ELF) electromagnetic propagation. (The pros and cons of this hypothesis are discussed in Section V of this paper.) In general, the entire field of paranormal research in the USSR is part of a larger one concerned with the interaction between electromagnetic fields and living organisms [411 [42] At the First International Congress on Parapsychology and Psychotronics in Prague, Czechoslovakia, in 1973, for example, Kholodov spoke at length about the susc(@ptibifity of living systems to extremely low-level ac and dc fields. lie de@,Tibed conditioning effects on the behavior of fish resulting fr,)m the application of 10 to 100 MW of RF to their tank 1431 11c USSR take these data seriously in that the Sovirt safety re- quirements for steady-state micro@xave exposurr set liinit@ 2 at 10 JJW/cm ,whereas the United States has set a stead,, r state limit of 10 mW!cm 2 [441. Kholodov spoke also about the nonthermal effects of microwaves on animals' central nervoii@ systems. His experiments were very carefully carried r@iit and are characteristic of a new dimension in paranormal research The increasing importance of this area in Soviet ieseaich was indicated recently when the Soviet Psychological Association issued an unprecedented position paper calling on the Soviet Academy of Sciences to step up efforts in this area [451 They recommended that the newly formed Psychological Institute within the Soviet Academy of Scien(es and the Psychological Institute of the Academy of Pedagogical S,-iences review the area and consider the creation of a new lab, @ratory within one of the institutes to study persons with nniisual abilities. They also recommended a comprehensive evakiation of experiments and theory by the A:@adejny of Science,@' Insti tute of Biophysics and Institute for the Problems of Infornia tion Transmission. The Soviet research, along with other behavionAicall,, oriented work, suggests that in addition to obtaining overt responses such as verbalizations or key presses from a sabject it should be possible to obtain objective evidence of infornia tion transfer by direct measurement of physiological pdrarrie- ters of a subject. Kamiya, Lindsle@, Pribram, Silverman, Walter, and others brought together to discuss physiological methods to detect ESP functioning, have suggested that a whole range of electroencephalogram (EFG) responses tich as evoked potentials (EP's), spontaneous EEG, and the contingent negative variation (CNV) might be sensitive indicators of' the detection of remote stimuli not mediated by usual sensory processes [461. Early experimentation of this type was carried b@, Douglas Dean at the Newark College of Engineeiing. In his (Since the 15-digit number (49.8 bits) actually was transinitted at fhe rate of 2.9 X 10-4 bit/s, an increase in bit rate by a fat tor of about 20 could be expected on the basis of a coding scl eme more optimum than that used in the experiments. See, foi example, Appendix A.) I Jr. Charles Tart at the University of California has written exi ensively on the so-called decline effect. He considers that ha, ing subjects attempt to guess cards, or perform any other rer etitious task for which they receive no feedback, follows tht classical technique for deconditioning any response. He thi s considers card guessing "a technique for extinguishing ps@ chic functioning in the laboratory" [32]. lart's injunctions of the mid-sixties were being heeded at Maimonides Hospital, Brooklyn, NY, by a team of researchers that included Dr. Montague Ullman, who was director of reso,arch for the hospital; Dr. Stanley Krippner; and, later, Ch,irles Honorton. These three worked together for several years on experiments on the occurrence of telepathy in dreams. In the course of a half-dozen experimental series, they found in their week-long sessions a number of subjects who had dreams that consistently were highly descriptive of pictorial ma i.erial that a remote sender was looking at throughout the nigat. This work is described in detail in the experimenters' book Dream Telepathy [331. Honorton is continuing work of his free-response type in which the subject has no precon- cei red idea as to what the target may be. I.i his more recent work with subjects in the waking state, Ho iorton is providing homogeneous stimulation to the subject wh ) is to describe color slides viewed by another person in a rer7iote room. In this new work, the subject listens to white noiie via earphones and views an homogeneous visual field imposed through the use of Ping-Pong ball halves to cover the su@@iect's eyes in conjunction with diffuse ambient illumina- tio t. In this so-called Ganzfeld setting, subjects are again able, now in the waking state, to give correct and often highly acL irate descriptions of the material being viewed by the sen ler [341. lit Honorton's work and elsewhere, it apparently has been the step away from the repetitive forced-choice experiment thai has opened the way for a wide variety of ordinary people to lemonstrate significant functioning in the laboratory, with- oui being bored into a decline effect. I his survey would be incomplete if we did not indicate cer ain aspects of the current state of research in the USSR. It is clear from translated documents and other sources [351 tha' many laboratories in the USSR are engaged in paranormal rescarch. ince the 1930's, in the laboratory of L. Vasiliev (Leningrad Ins itute for Brain Research), there has been an interest in the use of telepathy as a method of influencing the behavior of a pei ion at a distance. In Vasiliev's book Experiments in Mental Su@ gestion, he makes it very clear that the bulk of his labora- tOTv's experiments were aimed at long-distance communica- tio i combined with a form of behavior modification; for ex@ mple, putting people at a distance to sleep through hyp- no: is [ 361. Approved For Release 2003/06/24: CIA-:RDP -00999AO00300060005-1 L u L_.J 3., t Approved For Release 2003/06/24 CIA-RDP79-00999AGOM806000514 IEEE, MARCH 1976 se; rch for physiological correlates of information transfer, he us @d the plethysmograph to measure changes in the blood voiume in a finger, a sensitive indicator of autonomic nervous sy,,tem functioning [471. A plethysmographic measurement w., s made on the finger of a subject during telepathy experi- m@ ints. A sender looked at randomly selected target cards consisting of names known to the subject, together with names ur known to him (selected at random from a telephone book). TI e names of the known people were contributed by the sub- je(t and were to be of emotional significance to him. Dean fo ind significant changes in the chart recording of finger bl)od volume when the remote sender was looking at those names known to the subject as compared with those names raadomly chosen. 'Three other experiments using the physiological approach have now been published. The first work by Tart [48 1, a later work by Lloyd [491, and most recently the work by the authors [41 all follow a similar procedure. Basically, a subject is closeted in an electrically shielded room while his LEG is reL.orded. Meanwhile, in another laboratory, a second person is stimulated from time to time, and the time of that stimulus is marked on the magnetic-tape recording of the subject's EEG. The subject does not know when the remote stimulus periods are as compared with the nonstimulus periods. With regard to choice of stimulus for our own experimenta- tion, we noted that in previous work others had atterapted, without success, to detect evoked potential changes in a sub- ject's EEG in response to a single stroboscopic flash stimulus observed by another subject [501. In a discussion of that experiment, Kamiya suggested that because of the unknown temporal characteristics of the information channel, it might be more appropriate to use repetitive bursts of light to increase th,.- probability of detecting information transfer [ 5 11. There- fo-e, in our study we chose to use a stroboscopic flash train of IC-s duration as the remote stimulus. n the design of the study, we assumed that the application of the remote stimulus would result in responses similar to th )se obtained under conditions of direct stimulation. For eximple, when an individual is stimulated with a low- fr(quency (< 30 Hz) flashing light, the EEG typically shows a decrease in the amplitude of the resting rhythm and a driving of the brain waves at the frequency of the flashes [5 2 ]. Wo hypothesized that if we stimulated one subject in this mitriner (a putative sender), the EEG of another subject in a remote room with no flash present (a receiver) might show changes in alpha (9-11 Hz) activity and possibly an EEG drtving similar to that of the sender, or other coupling to the sender's EEG [531. The receiver was seated in a visually opaque, acoustically and electrically shielded, double-walled StLel room about 7 m from the sender. The details of the experiment, consisting of seven runs of thirty-six 10-s trials ea,h (twelve periods each for 0-Hz, 6-11z, and 16-Hz stimuli, ratidomly intermixed), are presented in [4]. This experiment pr@)ved to be successful. The receiver's alpha activity (9-11 Hz) sh )wed a significant reduction in average power (-24 percent, p '_ 0.04) and peak power (-28 percent, p < 0.03) during 10-Hz flash stimuli as compared with periods of no-flash stimulus. [A similar response was observed for 6-Hz stimuli (- 12 percent in average power, - 21 percent in peak power), but the latter result did not reach statistical significance.] Fi i@. 2 shows an overlay of three averaged EEG spectra from ore of the subject's 36 trial runs, displaying differences in al,.ha activity during the three stimulus conditions. Extensive cclitrol procedures were undertaken to determine if these t:! ui (D < l-_ _J 0 > Fig. 2. Occipital EEG frequency spectra, 0-20 fiz, of one subject (H.H.) acting as receiver showing amplitude changes in the 9-1 1-Hz band as a function of strobe frequency. Three cases: 0 , 6-, and I 6-Hz flashes (twelve trial averages). results were produced by system artifacts, electromagnetic pickup (EMI), or subtle cueing; the results were negative [4] @ As part of the experimental protocol, the subject was asked to indicate a conscious assessment for each trial (via telegraph key) as to the nature of the stimulus-, analysis showed these guesses to be at chance. Thus arousal as evidenced by signifi- cant alpha blocking occurred only at the noncognitivV level of physiological response. Hence the experiment provided direct physiological (EEG) evidence of perception of remote stimuli even in the absence of overt cognitive response. Whereas in out- experiments we used a remote light flash as a stimulus, Tart [481 in his work used an electrical shock to himself as sender, and Lloyd [49] simply told the sender to think of a red triangle each time a red warning light was illuminated within his view. Lloyd observed a consistent evoked potential in his subjects; whereas in our experiments and in Tart's, a reduction in amplitude and a desynchroniza- tion of alpha was observed-an arousal response. (If a subject is resting in an alpha-dominant condition and he is then stimulated, for example in any direct manner, one will observe a desynchronization and decrease in alpha power.) We con- sider that these combined results are evidence for the existence of noncognitive awareness of remote happenings and that they have a profound implication for paranormal research. Ill. SRI INVESTIGATIONS ot@ REMOTE VIFIWING Experimentation in remote viewing began during studies carried out to investigate the abilities of a New York artist, Ingo Swann, when he expressed the opinion that the insights gained during experiments at SRI had strengthened his ability (verified in other research before he joined the SRI program) to view remote locations [54] . To test Mr. Swann's asser- tion, a pilot study was set up in which a series of targets from around the globe were supplied by SRI personnel to the ex- perimenters on a double-blind basis. Mr. Swann's apparent ability to describe correctly details of buildings, roads, bridges, and the like indicated that it may be possible for a subject by means of mental imagery to access and describe randomly chosen geographical sites located several miles from the subject's position and demarcated by Some dppTO- priate means. Therefore, we set up a research program to test the remote-viewing hypothesis under rigidly controlled scientific conditions. In carrying out this program, we concentrated on what we considered to be our principal responsibility-to resolve under unambiguous conditions the basic issue of whether or not this Approved For Release J06/06/24: CIA-RDP79-00999AO00300060005-1 5 Hz 10 Hz 15 Hz PUT iOFF AND TARG AMRyPM%A"Ml?kXqW)M -,PW1w9-7OQ999AO00300060005-1 0 clas@ of paranormal perception phenomenon exists. At all timcs, we and others responsible for the overall program took mea;ures to prevent sensory leakage and subliminal cueing and to I revent deception, whether intentional or unintentional. Tocrisure evaluations independent of belief structures of both exp, rimenters and judges, all experiments were carried out und @r a protocol, described below, in which target selection at the beginning of experiments and blind judging of results at the end of experiments were handled independently of the rese irchers engaged in carrying out the experiments. Si( subjects, designated Sl through S6, were chosen for the stu6y. Three were considered as gifted or experienced subjects (S I through S3), and three were considered as learners (S4 t@r(,ugh S6). The a priori dichotomy between gifted and Iear,iers was based on the experienced group having heen suci essful in other studies conducted before this program and the learners group being inexperienced with regard to rar@,normal experimentation. Tite study consisted of a series of double-blind tests with l:)ciil targets in the San Francisco Bay Area so that several in- clep,,-ndent judges could visit the sites to establish docunwrita- tioii. The protocol was to closet the subject with an experi- riiej,ter at SRI and at an agreed-on time to obtain froin the subJect a description of an undisclosed remote site heing ,,isiied by a target team. In each of the experiments, one of the six program subjects served as remote-viewing subject, and SRI experimenters served as a target demarcation team at the remote location chosen in a double-blind protocol as tollows. In each experiment, SRI management randomly chose a target location from a list of targets within a 30-min driving tim,- from SRI; the target location selected was kept blind to subject and experimenters. The target pool consisted of more than 100 target locations chosen from a target-rich environ- ment. (Before the experimental series began, the Director of the Information Science and Engineering Division, not other- wise. associated with the experiment, established the set oflo- cations as the target pool which remained known only to him. Thu target locations were printed on cards sealed in envelopes and kept in the SRI Division office safe. They were available only with the personal assistance of the Division Director who issued a single random-number selected target card that con- stit ited the traveling orders for that experiment.) lit detail: To begin the experiment, the subject was closeted wita an experimenter at SRI to wait 30 min before beginning a narrative description of the remote location. A second ex- perimenter then obtained from the Division Director a target location from a set of traveling orders previously prepared and randomized by the Director and kept under his control. The target demarcation team, consisting of two to four SRI experi- meriters, then proceeded by automobile directly to the target without any communication with the subject or experimenter remaining behind. The experimenter remaining with the sub- ject at SRI was kept ignorant of both the particular target and the target pool so as to eliminate the possibility of cueing (ov@@rt or subliminal) and to allow him freedom in questioning the subject to clarify his descriptions. The demarcation team remained at the target site for an agreed-on 15-min period fohowing the 30 min allotted for travel .4 During the observa- 4fhe first subject (Sl) was allowed 30 min for his descriptioiis, but it v as found that he fatigued and bad little comment after the first 15 miy . The viewing time was therefore reduced to 15 min for subjects S2 hrough S6. Approved For Release 2003/06/24. 335 tion period, the remote-viewing subject was asked to describe his impressions of the target site into a tape recorder and to make any drawings he thought appropriate. An informal com- parison was then made when the demarcation team returned, and the subject was taken to the site to provide feedback. A. Subject SL Experienced To begin the series, Pat Price, a former California police com- missioner and city councilman, participated as a subject in nine experiments. In general, Price's ability to describe correctly buildings, docks, roads, gardens, and the like, includ- ing structural materials, color, ambience, and activity -often in great detail- indicated the functioning of a remote per- ceptual ability. A Hoover Tower target, for example, was recognized and named by name. Nonetheless, in general, the descriptions contained inaccuracies as well as correct state- ments. A typical example is indicated by the subject's drawing shown in Fig. 3 in which he correctly described a park-like area containing two pools of water one rectangular, 60 by 89 ft (actual dimensions 75 by 100 ft); the other circular, diameter 120 ft (actual diameter I 10 ft). lie incorrectly indi- cated the function, however, as water filtration rather than recreational swimming. (We often observe essentially correct descriptions of basic elements and patterns coupled with in- complete or erroneous analysis of function.) As can be seen from his drawing, he also included some elements, such as the tanks shown in the upper right, that are not present at the target site. We also note an apparent left-right reversal, often observed in paranormal perception experiments. To obtain a numerical evaluation of the accuracy of the remote-viewing experiment, the experimental results were subjected to independent judging on a blind basis by an SRI research analyst not otherwise associated with the research. The subject's response packets, which contained the nine typed unedited transcripts of the tape-recorded narratives along with any associated drawings, were unlabeled and pre- sented in random order. While standing at each target loca- tion, visited in turn, the judge was required to blind rank order the nine packets on a scale I to 9 (best to worst match). The statistic of interest is the sum of ranks assigned to the target- associated transcripts, lower values indicating better matches, For nine targets, the sum of ranks could range from nine to eighty-one. The probability that a given sum of ranks s or less will occur by chance is given by [ 5 51 (n) (i - N11- 1) Pr (s or less) = N-n L i=n 1=0 I n - where s is obtained sum of ranks, N is number of assignable ranks, n is number of occasions on which rankings were made, and 1 takes on values from zero to the least positive integer k in (i - n)ln. (Table I is a table to enable easy application of the above formula to those cases in which N = n.) The sum in this case, which included seven direct hits out of the nine, was 16 (see Table 11), a result significant at p = 2.9 X 10-5 by exact calculation. In Experiments 3, 4, and 6 through 9, the subject was se- cured in a double-walled copper-screen Faraday cage. The Faraday cage provides 120-dB attenuation for plane-wave radio-frequency radiation over a range of 15 kHz to 1 GHz_ For magnetic fields, the attenuation is 68 dB at 15 kHz and decreases to 3 d B at 60 Hz. The results of rank order judging (Table 11) indicate that the use of Faraday cage electrical CIA-RDP79-00999AO00300060005-1 336 Approved For Release 20PJ/06/24 -RPP7_9_-00999AVVG30M000" IEEE, MARCH 1976 I ;t Iloso _LJ AV E. ELECT. TENNIS COURTS DIST. Ir. - 75 PLAY 100 GROUNDS POOL HOUSE SERVICE YARD t CONCRETE BLOCK FIRE STATION 4ti7 'PICNII AREA/ @STATX@ IQ\ (a) (b) Fig. 3. Swimming pool complex as remote-viewing target. (a) City map of target location. (b) Drawing by Price (Si). TABLE I CRITICAL VALUES OF SUMS OF RANKS FOR PREFERENTIAL MATCHING Number of Assignable Probability (one-tailed) that the Indicated Sum of Ranks or Less Would Occur by (@hau- - - Ranks (N) 0 - NT O .1 C@ 0.05 0.04 0.025 0.01 0.005 0.002 0.001 0.0005 10- 10- 4 7 6 5 5 5 4 4 5 11 10 9 8 8 7 6 6 5 5 6 16 15 13 13 12 11 10 9 8 1 6 7 22 20 18 18 17 15 14 12 12 1, q 8 29 27 24 24 22 20 19 t7 16 13 11 9 8 9 37 34 31 30 29 26 24 22 21 20 17 14 1') 10 10 46 42 39 38 36 33 31 29 27 25 22 19 16 1, 11 56 51 48 47 45 41 38 36 34 32 28 24 2J 17 67 61 58 56 54 49 47 43 41 39 35 30 2 22 ivore: imS fame applies only to tliose special cases in which the number of occasions on which objects are being ranked (n) is equal to the number of assignable ranks (N). Each entry represents the largest number that is significant at the indicated p-level. Source: R. L. Morris [55 ]. shieldirig does not prevent high-quality descriptions from being B. Subject S4: Learner obtained. This experiment was designed to be a replication of our pre- As a backup judging procedure, a panel of five additional vious experiment with Price, the first replication attempted. SRI scientists not othprwise associated with the research were The subject for this experiment was Mrs. Hella Haininid, a asked simply to blind match the unedited typed transcripts gifted professional photographer. She was selected for this (with associated drawings) generated by the remote viewer series on the basis of her successful performance as a per- against the nine target locations which they independently cipient in the EEG experiment described earlier. Outside of visited in turn. The transcripts were unlabeled and presented that interaction, she had no previous experience with apparent in random order. A correct match consisted of a transcript paranormal functioning. of a given date being matched to the target of that date. In- At the'time we began working with Mrs. Hammid, she had stead of the expected number of I match each per judge, the no,strong feelings about the of her ability to suc- number of correct matches obtained by the fivejudges was 7, ceed in this task. This was in contrast to both Ingo Swann 6, 5, 3, and 3, respectively. Thus, rather than the expected who had come to our laboratory fresh from a lengthy and total number of 5 correct matches from the judges, 24 such apparently successful series of experiments with Dr. Gertrude mal ches were obtained. Schmeidler at City College of New York [56] and Pat Price Approved For Release 2003/06/24; W79-00999AO00300060005-1 PUTHOFF AND TARG 0300060005-1 337 PEDESTRIAN OVERPASS TARGET Fig. 4. Subject Hammid (S4) drawing, described as "some kind of diagonal trough up in the air." TABLE11 DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS ASSIGNED To TRANSCRIPTS ASSOCIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION FOR ExPERIENCED SUBJECT PRICE (SI) Target Location Distance Rank o I Assori. ted Transcript Hoover Tower, Stanford 3.4 1 Baylands Nature Preserve, Palo Alto 6.4 1 Radio telescope, Portola Valley 6.4 1 Marina, Redwood City 6.8 1 Bridge toll plaza, Fremont 14.5 6 Drive-in theater, Palo Alto 5.1 1 Arts and Crafts Plaza, Menlo Park 1.9 1 Catholic Church, Portola Valley 8.5 3 Swiming pool complex, Palo Alto 3.4 1 Total sum of ranks 16 (p=2.9Y'l0` who felt that he used his remote-viewing ability in his every- day life. In comparison with the latter two, many people are more influenced by their environment and are reluctant under public scrutiny to attempt activities that are generally thought to be impossible. Society often provides inhibition and nega- tive feedback to the individual who might otherwise have explored his own nonregular perceptual ability. We all share an historical tradition of "the stoning of prophets and the burning of witches" and, in more modern times, the hospitaliza- tion of those who claim to perceive things that the majority do not admit to seeing. Therefore, in addition to maintaining Approved For Release 2003/06/24: scientific rigor, one of our primary tasks as researchers is to provide an environment in which the subject feels safe to explore the possibility of paranormal perception. With a new subject, we also try to stress the nonuniqueness of the ability because from our experience paranormal functioning appears to be a latent ability that all subjects can articulate to some degree. Because of Mrs. Hammid's artistic background, she was ca- pable of drawing and describing visual images that she could not identify in any cognitive or analytic sense. When the target demarcation team went to a target location which was a pedestrian overpass, the subject said that she saw "a kind of trough up in the air," which she indicated in the upper part of her drawing in Fig. 4. She went on to explain, "If you stand where they are standing you will see something like this," indicating the nested squares at the bottom of Fig. 4. As it turned out, a judge standing where she indicated would have a view closely resembling what she had drawn, as can be seen from the accompanying photographs of the target loca- tion. It needs to be emphasized, however, that judges did not have access to our photographs of the site, used here for illustrative purposes only, but rather they proceeded to each of the target locations by list. In another experiment, the subject described seeing "an open barnlike structure with a pitched roof." She also saw a "kind of slatted side to the structure making light and dark bars on the wall." Her drawing and a photograph of the associated bicycle shed target are shown in Fig. 5. (Subjects are encouraged to make drawings of anything they visualize and associate with the remote location because drawings they make are in general more accurate than their verbal description.) As in the original series with Price, the results of the nine- CIA-RDR79-00999A000300060005-1 338 Approved For Release 2.003/06/24: CIA-RDP79-Q0999Aqqg?PMq90A_THE IEEE, MARCH j976 Fig. S. Subject Hammid (S4) response to bicycle shed target described as an open "barn-like building" with "slats on the sides" and a "pitched roof." TABLE III DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGs AssIGNEDTo TRANSCRIPTS AssocIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION FOR LEARNER SUBJECT HAMMID (S4) Target Location Distance (km) Rank of A--i@ted Trans, ript Methodist Church, Palo Alto 1.9 1 Ness Auditorium, Menlo Park 0.2 i Merry-go-round, Palo Alto 3.4 Parking garage, Mountain View 8.1 2 SRI International Courtyard, Menlo Park 0.2 L Bicycle shed, Menlo Park 0.1 2 Railroad trestle bridge, Palo Alto 1.3 2 Pumpkin patch, Menlo Park 1.3 1 Pedestrian overpass, ?.I. Alto 5.0 2 Total sm. of ranks 13 experiment series were submitted for independent judging on a blind basis by an SRI research analyst not otherwise associ- ated with the research. While at each target location, visited in turn, the judge was required to blind rank order the nine unedited typed manuscripts of the tape-recorded narratives, along with any associated drawings generated by the remote viewer, on a scale I to 9 (best to worst match). The sum of ranks assigned to the target-associated transcripts in this case was 13, a result significant at p = 1.8 X 10-6 by exact calcula- tion (see Table I and discussion), and included five direct hits and four second ranks (Table 111). Again, as a backup judging procedure, a panel of five addi- tional judges not otherwise associated with the research were asked simply to blind match the unedited typed tTan- scripts and associated drawings generated by the remote viewer, against the nine target locations which they independently visited in turn. A correct match consisted of a transcript of a given date being matched to the target of that date. In- stead of the expected number of I match each per judge, the number of correct matches obtained by the five judges was 5, 3, 3, 2, and 2, respectively. Thus, rather than the ex- pected total number of 5 correct matches from the judges, 15 such matches were obtained. C. Subjects S2 and S3. Experienced Having completed a series of 18 remote-viewing experiments, 9 each with experienced subject S I (Price) and learner S 4 (Hammid), additional replication experiments, four with each subject, were carried out with experienced subjects S 2 (Elgin) and S 3 (Swann) and learners S 5 and S 6. To place the judging on a basis comparable to that used with S I and S4, the four transcripts each of experienced subjects S 2 and S 3 were com- bined into a group of eight for rank order judging to be com- pared with the similarly combined results of the learners S5 and S6. The series with S2 (Elgin, an SRI research analyst) provided a further example of the dichotomy between verbal and draw- ing responses. (As with medical literature, case histories often are more illuminating than the summary of results.) The ex- periment described here was the third conducted with this Approved For Release 2003106/24: CIA-RDP79-00999AO00300060005-1 BICYCLE SHED TARGET DETAIL OF BICYCLE SHED PUTHOFF AND TARGAYMW"f?'dAF&4FikM&sWl2MWM2T4()N2A6ktyib?9-00999AO00300060005-1 339 Fig. 6. Subject Elgin (S2) drawings in response to tennis court target, subject. It was a demonstration experiment for a government visitor who had heard of our work and wanted to evaluate our experimental protocol. In the laboratory, the subject, holding a bearing compass at arm's length, began the experiment by indicating the direction of the target demarcation team correctly to within 50. (in all four experiments with this subject, he has always been within 100 of the correct direction in this angular assessment.) The subject then generated a 15-min tape-recorded description and the drawings shown in Fig, 6. In discussing the drawings, Elgin indicated that he was uncertain as to the action, but had the impression that the demarcation team was located at a museum (known to him) in a particular park. In fact, the target was a tennis court lo- cated in that park about 90 m from the indicated museum. Once again, we note the characteristic (discussed earlier) of a resemblance between the target site and certain gestalt ele- ments of the subject's response, especially in regard to the drawings, coupled with incomplete or erroneous analysis of the significances. Nonetheless, when rank ordering transcripts I through 8 at the site, the judge ranked this transcript as 2. This example illustrates a continuing observation that most of the correct information related to us by subjects is of a non- analytic nature pertaining to shape, form, color, and material rather than to function or name. A second example from this group, generated by S3 (Swann), indicates the level of proficiency that can be attained with practice. In the two years since we first started working with Swann, he has been studying the problem of separating the ex- ternal signal from the internal noise. In our most recent experiments, he dictates two lists for us to record. One list contains objects that he "sees," but does not think are located at the remote scene, A secor(' lis! contains objects that he thinks are at the scene. In our valuation, he has made much progress in this most essential ability to separate memory and imagination from paranormal inputs, This is the key to bringing the remote-viewing channel to fruition with regard to its potential usefulness. The quality of transcript that can be generated by this pro- cess is evident from the results of our most recent experiment with Swann. The target location chosen by the usual double- blind protocol was the Palo Alto City Hall- Swann described a tall building with vertical columns and "set in" windows. His sketch, together with the photograph of the site, is shown in Fig. 7. He said there was a fountain, "but I don't hear it." At the time the target team was at the City Hall during the experiment, the fountain was not running. He also made an effort to draw a replica of the designs in the pavement in front of the building, and correctly indicated the number of trees (four) in the sketch. For the entire series of eight, four each from S2 and S3, the numerical evaluation based on blind rank ordering of tran- scripts at each site was significant at p = 3.8 X 10-4 and in- cluded three direct hits and three second ranks for the target- associated transcripts (see Table IV). D. Subjects S5 and S6: Learners To complete the series, four experiments each were carried out with learner subjects S5 and S6, a man and woman on the SRI professional staff. The results in this case, taken as a Approved For Release 2003/06/24: CIA-RDP79-00999AO00300060005-1 TARGET--TENNIS COURTS 340 Approved For Release 20006/24 ClA-RDP79-00999AQMQ00&WT1iE IEEE, MARCH 1976 4L WU 0.0.6 tw 0@0..db UZ444 two &V" 0111A fo" q. Ara) - CAO% WA@^ coo A. Fig. 7. Subject Swann (S3) response to City Hall target group, did not differ significantly from chance. For the series of eight (judged as a group of seven since one target came up twice, once for each subject), the numerical evaluation based on blind rank ordering of transcripts at each site was non- significant at p = 0.08, even though there were two direct hits and two second ranks out of the seven (see Table V). One of the diredt hits, which occurred with subject S6 in her first experiment, provides an example of the "first-time effect" that has been rigorously explored and is well-known to experi- menters in the field [571. The outbound experimenter obtained, by random protocol from the pool, a target blind to the experimenter with the subject, as is our standard pro- cedure, and proceeded to the location. The subject, a mathe- matician in the computer science laboratory who had no pre- TABLE IV DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS AssIGNED To TRANSCRIPTS ASSOCIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION FOR EXPERIENCED SUBJECTS ELGIN (S2) AND SWANN (S3) Subject Distance Target Location (k.) Rank of Associated Transcript S2 BART Station (Transit System), Fremont 16.1 1 S2 Shielded room, SRI, Menlo Park 0.1 2 S2 Tennis court, Palo Alto 3.4 2 S2 Golf course bridge, Stanford 3.4 2 S3 City Hall, Palo Alto 2.0 1 S3 Miniature golf course, Menlo Park 3.0 1 S3 Kiosk in park, Menlo Park 0.3 3 S3 Baylands Nature Preserve, Palo Alto 6.4 3 T6tal sum of ranks 15 (p=3.8Xl0-4) TABLE V DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS ASSIGNED To TRANSCRIPTS ASSOCIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION FOR LEARNER SUBJECTS S5 AND S6 Subiect Tarp:et Location Distance (k.) Rank of As iated Tr:oncscript S5 Pedestrian overpass, Palo Alto 5.0 3 S5 Railroad trestle bridge, Palo Alto I @ 6 S5 Windmillp PorLola Valley 8.5 2 S5, S6 White Plaza, Stanford (2) 3.8 1 S6 Airport ' Palo Alt. 5.5 2 S6 Kiosk in Park , Menlo Park 0.3 5 S6 Boathouse, Stanford 4.0 1 Total am of ranks 20 (pzO.08, NS) vious experience in remote viewing, began to describe a large square with a fountain. Four minutes into the -.xperiment, she recognized the location and correctly identified it by name (see Fig. 8). (It should be noted that in the area from which the target locations were drawn there are other fountains as well, some of which were in the target pool.) As an ex- ample of the style of the narratives generated during remote viewing with inexperienced subjects and of the part played by the experimenter remaining with the subject in such a case, we have included the entire unedited text of this experiment as Appendix B. E. Normal and Paranormal: Use of Unselected Subjects in Remote Viewing After more than a year of following the experimental pro- tocol described above and observing that even inexperienced subjects generated results better than expected, we initiated a series of experiments to explore further whether individuals other than putative "psychics" can demonstrate the remote- viewing ability. To test this idea, we have a continuing pro- gram to carry out additional experiments of the outdoor type with new subjects whom we have no a priori reason to believe have paranormal perceptual ability. To date we have collected data from five experiments with two individuals in this cate- gory: a man and a woman who were visiting government scientists interested in observing our experimental protocols. The motivation for these particular experiments was twofold. First, the experiments provide data that indicate the level of proficiency that can be expected from unselected volunteers. Approved For Release 2PO3/06/24.: ClA-RDP79-00999A000300060005-1 r5 Am 7Y. ID40 &VA. ties? PUTHOFF AND TAR~PWROWYJFA?rCfNhi~eFgPQ~AW94licq4TRppr?-9-00999AO00300060005-1 341 Fig. 8. Subject (S6) drawing of White Plaza, Stanford University. Sub- ject drew what she called "curvy benches" and then announced cor- rectly that the place was "White Plaza at Stanford." Second, when an individual observes a successful demonstra tion experiment involving another person as subject, it inevi- tably occurs to him that perhaps chicanery is involved. We have found the most effective way to settle this issue for the observer is to have the individual himself act as a subject so as to obtain personal experience against which our ieported results can be evaluated. The first visitor (VI) was invited to participate as a subject in a three-experiment series. All three experiments contained elements descriptive of the associated target locations; the quality of response increased with practice. The third re- Sponse is shown in Fig. 9, where again the pattern elements in the drawing appeared to be a closer match than the subject's analytic interpretation of the target object as a cupola. Jl@ t//A) RESPONSES OF VISITING SCIENTIST SUBJECT Fig. 9. Subject (V 1) drawing of merry-go-round target. dL Approved For Release 2003/06124: CIA I'8DP79-00999AO00300060005-1 342 Approved For Release 2003106124: CIA-RDP79-00999A0M0G0WW5Rt_ 1EEF, MARCH 1976 TECHNOLOGY SERIES TYPEWRITER TARGET som, Y. mseGe poels &W Ut(-@b 011@f oj-d6o ddAgL - a, a 6f. itfft *,ocu IL23 71 atK V-q(W',V0A VW OWSOVA SUBJECT SWANN (S3) RESPONSE SUBJECT HAMMID (S4) RESPONSE Fig. 10. Drawings of a typewriter target by two subjects. The second visitor [V21 participated as a subject in two ex- periments. In his first experiment, he generated one of the higher signal-to-noise results we have observed. He began his narrative, "There is a red A-frame building and next to it is a large yellow thing [a tree-Editor]. Now further left there is another A-shape. It looks like a swing-set, but it is pushed down in a gully so I can't see the swings." [All cor- rect.] He then went on to describe a lock on the front door that he said "looks like it's made of laminated steel, so it must be a Master lock." [Also correct.] For the series of five-three from the first subject and two from the second-the numerical evaluation based on blind rank ordering of the transcripts at each site was significant at p =z 0.017 and included three direct hits and one second rank for the target-associated transcripts. (See Table VI.) TABLE V1 DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS ASSIGNED To TRANSCRIPTs AssocIATFD WIT" EAcm TARGET LoCATION FOR VISITOR SUBJFCTS V1 AND V2 b1-t__,_ Target Locati n (k.) Transcri V1 Bridg@ M-1. ',.,k I V1 Bayl-- Freser-, 1,1o it- 6@4 V1 Me-y-- i-d, P,1. All, 3- V2 Windmil@, P-L,la Valley -5 V2 Apartmert iwi@ing pool, m--t- i- q'i To L. I f , a.k, (p 0.017) Approved For Release 2003/06124: CIA,-RDP79.~,OP999AO00300060005-1 I PUTHOFF AND TARG: #wL9y@0LF(qQ@ftftaftMP99#W?& q"W79-00999AO00300060005-1 343 TARGET LOCATION: XEROX MACHINE (TECHNOLOGY SERIES) Fig. 11. Drawings by three subjects (S2, S3, and V3) for Xerox machine target. When asked to describe the square at upper left of response on 1: he right, subject (V3) said, "There was this predominant light source which might have been a window, and a working surface which might have been the sill, or a working surface or desk." Earlier the subject had said, "I have the feeling that there is something silhouetted against the window." Observations with unselected subjects such as those de- scribed above indicate that remote viewing may be a latent and widely distributed perceptual ability. F. Technology Series: Short-Range Remote Viewing Because remote viewing is a perceptual ability, we consid- ered it important to obtain data on its resolution capabilities. To accomplish this, we turned to the use of indoor techno- logical targets. Twelve experiments were carried out with five different sub- jects, two of whom were visiting government scientists. They were told that one of the experimenters would be sent by random protocol to a laboratory within the SRI complex and that he would interact with the equipment or apparatus at that location. It was further explained that the experimenter remaining with the subject was, as usual, kept ignorant ot the contents of the target pool to prevent cueing during question- ing. (Unknown to subjects, targets in the pool were used with replacement; one of the goals of this particular experiment was to obtain multiple responses to a given target to investigate whether correlation of a number of subject responses would provide enhancement of the signal-to-noise ratio.) The sub- ject was asked to describe the target both verbally (tape recorded) and by means of drawings during a time-synchronized 15-min interval in which the outbound experimenter inter- acted in an appropriate manner with the equipment iD the target area. In the twelve experiments, seven targets were used: a drill r- press, Xerox machine, video terminal, chart recorder, fou state random number generator, machine shop, and type- write.r. Three of these were used twice (drill press, video terminal, and typewriter) and one (Xerox machine) came up three times in our random Comparisons of the targets and subject drawings for three of the multiple-response cases (the typewriter, Xerox machine, and video terminal) are shown in Figs. 10, 11, and 12. As is apparent from these illustrations alone, the experiments provide circumstantial evidence for an information channel of useful bit rate. This includes experiments in which visit- ing government scientists participated as subjects (Xerox machine and video terminal) to observe the protocol. In general, it appears that use of multiple-subject responses to a single target provides better sign al-t o-n oisee ratio than target identification by a single individual. This conclusion is borne out by the judging described below. Given that in general the drawings constitute the most accurate portion of a subject's description, in the first judging procedure a judge was asked simply to blind match only the drawings (i.e., without tape transcripts) to the targets. Multiple- subject responses to a given target were stapled together, and thus seven subject-drawing response packets were to be matched to the seven different targets for which drawings were made. The judge did not have access to our photographs of the target locations, used for illustration purposes only, but rather proceeded to each of the target locations by list. While standing at each target location, the judge was required to rank order the seven subject-drawing response packets (presented in random order) on a scale I to 7 (best to worst match). For seven targets, the sum of ranks could. 7ange from 7 to 49. The sum in this case, which included I direct hit and 4 second ranks out of the 7 (see Table V11) was 18, a result significant at p = 0.036. In the second more detailed effort at evaluation, a visiting scientist selected at random one of the 12 data packages (a drill press experiment), sight unseen and submitted it for in- esti- for engineer with an request a selection procedure. dependent analysis to an n, Approved For Release 2003/06/24: @CIA-RPP79-00999AO00300060005-1 TO ADD INTEREST TO TARGET LOCATION EXPERIMENTER WITH HIS HEAD BEING XEROXED Approved For Release.2M3/06124: _QlA-RDR7,9-- __00999A000300060005-1 344 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 TARGET: VIDEO MONITOR FOR TEXT EDITING (TECHNOLOGY SERIES) F 2 (b) Fig. 12. Drawing by two subjects of a video monitor target. (a) Subject (S4) drawing of "box with light coming out of it ... painted flat black and in the middle of the room." (b) Second subject (V2) saw a computer terminal with relay racks in the background. TABLE VII DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS ASSIGNED TO SUBJECT DRAWINGS ASSOCIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION subject Target Rank of Associated Drawings S3, S4 Drill press 2 S2, S3, V3 Xerox machine 2 S4, V2 Video terminal I S3 Chart recorder 2 S4 Random number generator "I S4 Machine shop 3 S3, S4 Typewriter 2 Total am of ranks 18 (p=0,036) mate as to what was being described. The analyst, blind as to the target and given only the subject's taped narrative and drawing (Fig. 13), was able, from the subject's description alone, to correctly classify the target as a "man-sized vertical boring machine." G. Summary of Remote Viewing Results i) Discussion: The descriptions supplied by the subjects in the experiments involving remote viewing of natural targets or laboratory apparatus, although containing inaccuracies, were sufficiently accurate to permit the judges to differentiate among various targets to the degree indicated. A summary TABLE VIII SUMMARY: REMOTE VIEWING - ------ SUbj-t Numb- of Experiment. p-Value, Rank order Judging With ..tu-1 ta,g@t@ Sl (e.P-ic-j) 1 2.9 , LO 5 S2 and S3 (cxperienced) 8 3.8 , IC,- 4 S4 (learneri 9 1,8 x 10-6 S5 and S6 tt-ruers) 8 0.Q8 (NS) VI and V2 (learners/visitors) 0.017 With technology targets S2, S3, S4, V2, V3 12 0.036 tabulation of the statistical evaluations of these fifty-one ex- periments with nine subjects is presented in Table V111. The overall result, evaluated conservatively on the basis of a judging procedure that ignores transcript quality beyond that necessary to rank order the data packets (vastly underestimat- ing the statistical significance of individual descriptions), clearly indicates the presence of an information channel of useful bit rate. Furthermore, it appears that the principal difference between experienced subjects and inexperienced volunteers is not that the latter never exhibit the faculty, but rather that their results are simply less reliable, more sporadic. Nevertheless, as described earlier, individual transcripts from the inexperienced group of subjects number among some of the best obtained. Such observations indicate a hypothesis that remote viewing may be a latent and widely distributed perceptual ability. Approved For Release 2003/06/24 :,CIA-R ,t)P7"0999AO00300060005-1 UPW '44' PUTHOFF AND TAROF R_VA1lFAqrc4qM!je@90QAkWA4-r. ATI~Pfi~kg-90.999AO00300060005-1 BELT DRIVE FOR DRILL PRESS (CAN BE SEEN ONLY FROM ABOVE MACHINE) Fig. 13. Subject (S4) drawing of drill press showing belt dri@e, stool, and a "vertical graph that goes up and down," Thus the primary achievement of the SRI program was the elicitation of high-quality remote viewing from individuals who agreed to act as subjects. Criticism of this claim could in principle be put forward on the basis of three potential flaws. 1) The study could involve naivet6 in protocol that permits various forms of cueing, intentional or unintentional. 2) The experiments discussed could be selected out of a larger pool of experiments of which many are of poorer quality. 3) Data for the reported experiments could be edited to show only the matching elements, the nonmatching elements being discarded. All three criticisms, however, are invalid. First, with regard to cueing, the use of double-blind protocols ensures that none of the persons in contact with the subject can be aware of the target. Second, selection of experiments for reporting did not take place-, every experiment was entered as performed on a master log and is included in the statistical evaluations. Third, data associated with a given experiment remain unedited-1 all experiments are tape recorded and all data are included un- edited in the data package to be judged and evaluated. 345 In the process of judging-attempting to niatch transcripts against targets on the basis of the information in the transcripts -some patterns and regularities in the transcript descriptions became evident, particularly regarding individual styles in remote viewing and in the perceptual form of the descriptions given by the subjects, These patterns and the judging procedure are discussed below a) Styles of response: The fifty-one trariscrirts were taken from nine different subjects. Comparing the tran- scripts of one subject with those of another revealed that each pattern tended to focus on certain aspects of the remote target complex and to exclude others, so that each had an individual pattern of response, like a signature. Subject S3, for example, frequently responded with topo- graphical descriptions, maps, and architectural features of the target locations. Subject S2 often focused on the behavior of the remote experimenter or the sequence of actions he carried out at the target. The transcripts of subject S4, more than those of other subjects, had descriptions of the feel of the lo- cation, and experiential or sensory gestalts-for example, light/dark elements in the scene and indoor/outdoor and enclosed/open distinctions. Prominent features of S I Is tran- scripts were detailed descriptions of what the target persons were concretely experiencing, seeing, or doing -for example, standing on asphalty blacktop overlooking water-, looking at a purple iris. The range of any individual subject's responses was wide. Anyone might draw a map or describe the mood of the remote experimenter, but the consistency of each subject's overall approach suggests that just as individual descriptions of a directly viewed scene would differ, so these diffefcrice-, also occur in remote-viewing processes b) Nature of the description: The concrete des, lipLions that appear most commonly in transciJpt,,, are at tlj_ level of subunits of the overall scene. For example, when iliv target was a Xerox copy machine, the responses inJudc,l (S2) a rolling object ('the moving light) or dial,. and i cover that is lifted (S3), but the machine as a whole ww, not identified by name or function. In a few transcripts, the subjects correctly identified and named the target. In the case of a computef terminal, the subject (V2) apparently perceived the terminal and the relly racks behind it. in the case of targets which were Hoover 'Tower and Whit-, Plaza, the subjects (S I and S6, respectively) seemed to identify the locations through analysis of their initial images of the elements of the target. There were also occasional incorrect identifications. Gestalts were incorrectly named; for example, swimming pools in a park were identified as water storage tanks at a water filtration plant (S I). The most common perceptual level was thus an intermediate one--the individual elements and items that make up the tar- get. This is suggestive of a scanning process that takes sample perceptions from within the overall environment. When the subjects tried to make sense out of these fragmen- tary impressions, they often resorted to metaphors or con- structed an image with a kind of perceptual inference. From a feeling of the target as an "august" and "solemn" building, a subject (S4) said it might be a library; it was a church. A pedestrian overpass above a freeway was described as a conduit (S4). A rapid transit station, elevated above the countryside, was associated with an observatory (S2). These responses seem to be the result of attempts to process partial informa- Approved For Release 2003/06/24: CIAR"'b79-00999AO00300060005-1 TARGET: DRILL PRESS JTECHNOLOGY SERIES) 346 Approved For Release, 2003/0!R/24: q[A-RPPYV00999AO00300060005-1 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 tion: similarly, this occurs in other parapsychological experi- ments. These observations are compatible with the hypotheses that information received in a putative remote-viewing mode is processed piecemeal in pattern form (consistent with a low bit rate process, but not necessarily requiring it); and the errors arise in the processes of attempted integration of the data into larger patterns directed toward verbal labeling. When the subjects augmented the verbal transcripts with drawings or sketches, these often expressed the target elements more accurately than the verbal descriptions. Thus the draw- ings tended to correspond to the targets more clearly and precisely than the words of the transcript. The descriptions given by the subjects sometimes went be- yond what the remote experimenter experienced, at least con- sciously. For example, one subject (S4) described and drew a belt drive at the top of a drill press that was invisible even to the remote experimenter who was operating the machine; another subject (SI) described a number of items behind shrubbery and thus not visible to members of the demarcation team at the site. Curiously, objects in motion at the remote site were rarely mentioned in the transcript. For example, trains crossing the railroad trestle target were not described, though the remote experimenter stood very close to them. Also in a few cases, the subject descriptions were inaccurate regarding size of structures. A 20-ft courtyard separating two buildings was described as 200 ft wide, and a small shed was expanded to a barn-like structure. c) Blind judging of transcripts: The judging procedure entailed examining the transcripts for a given experimental series and attempting to match the transcripts with the cor- rect targets on the basis of their correspondences. The tran- scripts varied from coherent and accurate descriptions to mix- tures of correspondences and noncorrespondences. Since the judge did not know a priori which elements of the descriptions were correct or incorrect, the task was complicated, and tran- scripts often seemed plausibly to match more than one target. A confounding factor in these studies is that some target lo- cations have similarities that seem alike at some level of per- ception. For example, a radio telescope at the top of a hill, the observation deck of a tower, and a jetty on the edge of a bay all match a transcript description of "looking out over a long di -stance." A lake, a fountain, and a creek may all result in an image of water for the subject. Therefore, in several cases, even correct images may not help in the conservative differential matching procedure used. According to the judge, the most successful procedure was a careful element-by-element comparison that tested each tran- script against every target and used the transcript descriptions and drawings as arguments for or against assigning the tran- script to a particular target. In most cases, this resulted in either a clear conclusion or at least a ranking of probable matches; these matches were subjected to the statistical analyses presented in this paper. 2) Summary: In summary, we do not yet have an under- standing of the nature of the inform ation-bearing signal that a subject perceives during remote viewing. The subjects com- monly report that they perceive the signal visually as though they were looking at the object or place from a position in its immediate neighborhood. Furthermore, the subjects' per- ceptual viewpoint has mobility in that they can shift their point of view so as to describe elements of a scene that would not be visible to an observer merely standing at ground level and describing what he sees. (In particular, a @ubject often correctly describes elements not visible to the target demarca- tion team.) Finally, motion is seldom reported; in fact, moving objects often are unseen even when nearby static objects are correctly identified. A comparison of the results of remote viewing (a so-called free-response task) with results of forced-choice tasks, such as the selection of one of four choices generated by a random number generator 158 1, reveals the following findings. From a statistical viewpoint, a subject is more likely to describe, with sufficient accuracy to permit blind matching, a remote site chosen at random than he is to select correctly one of four random numbers. Our experience with these phenomena leads us to consider that this difference in task performance may stem from fundamental signal-to-noise considerations. Two principal sources of noise in the systern apparently are mem- ory and imagination, both of which can give rise to mental pictures Of greater clarity than the target to be perceived. In the random number task, a subject can create a perfect mental picture of each of the four possible outputs in his own imagi- nation and then attempt to obtain the correct answer by a mental matching operation. The same is true for card guessing experiments. On the other hand, the subject in remote view- ing is apparently more likely to approach the task with a blank mind as he attempts to perceive pictorial information from remote locations about which he may have no stored mental data. Finally, we observe that most of the correct information that subjects relate to us is of a nonanalytic nature pertaining to shape, form, color, and material rather than to function or name. In consultation with Dr. Robert Ornstein of the Langley- Porter Neuropsychiatric Institute, San Francisco, CA, and with Dr. Ralph Kiernan of the Department of Neurology, Stanford University Medical Center, Stanford, CA, we have formed the tentative hypothesis that paTanormal functioning may involve specialization characteristic of the brain's right hemi- sphere. This possibility is derived from a variety of evidence from clinical and neurosurgical sources which indicate that the two hemispheres of the human brain are specialized for dif- ferent cognitive functions. The left hemisphere is predomi- nantly active in verbal and other analytical functioning and the right hemisphere predominates in spatial and other holistic processing [591, [601. Further research is necessary to elucidate the relationship between right hemisphere function and paranormal abilities. Nonetheless, we can say at this point that the remote-viewing results of the group of subjects at SRI have characteristics in common with more familiar performances that require right hemispheric function. The similarities include the highly schematicized drawings of ob- jects in a room or of remote scenes. Verbal identification of these drawings is often highly inaccurate and the drawings themselves are frequently left-right reversed relative to the target configuration. Further, written material generally is not cognized. These characteristics have been seen in left brain-injured patients and in call osal -se c tioned patients. As a result of the above considerations, we have learned tc urge our subjects simply to describe what they see as opposed to what they think they are looking at. We have learned that their unanalyzed perceptions are almost always a better guide to the true target than their interpretations of the perceived data. Approved For Release 2003/06/24: CIA-RDPA-00999A000300060005-1 . @ tl_ PUTHOFF AND TAIM. MYROrFRE MR§RtMNg§Q&if6l4Ar-v4jt-)0999AO00300060005-1 347 IV. CONSIDERATIONS CONCERNING TIME If the authors may be forgiven a personal note, we wish to express that this section deals with observations that we have been reluctant to publish because of their striking apparent in- compatibility with existing concepts. The motivating factor for presenting the data at this time is the ethical consideration that theorists endeavoring to develop models for paranormal functioning should be apprised of all the observable data if their efforts to arrive at a comprehensive and correct descrip- tion are to be successful. During the course of the experimentation in remote viewing (Section 111), subjects occasionally volunteered the informa- tion that they had been thinking about their forthcoming par- ticipation in a remote-viewing experiment and had an image come to them as to what the target location was to be. On these occasions, the information was given only to the experi- menter remaining at SRI with the subject and was unknown to the outbound experimenter until completion of the experi- ment. Two of these contributions were among the most accurate descriptions turned in during those experiments. Since the target location had not yet been selected when the subject communicated his perceptions about the target, we found the data difficult to contend with. We offer these spontaneous occurrences not as proof of pre- cognitive perception, but rather as the motivation that led us to do further work in this field. On the basis of this firsthand evidence, together with the copious literature describing years of precognition experiments carried out in various other labo- ratories, we decided to determine whether a subject could per- form a perceptual task that required both spatial and temporal remote viewing. It is well known and recently has been widely discussed that nothing in the fundamental laws of physics forbids the appar- ent transmission of information from the future to the present (discussed further in Section V). Furthermore, there is a gen- eral dictum that "in physical law, everything that is not forbid- den, is required" [61 ]. With this in mind, we set out to con- duct very well-controlled experiments to determine whether we could deliberately design and execute experiments for the sole purpose of observing precognition under laboratory conditions. The experimental protocol was identical to that followed in previous remote-viewing experiments with but one exception. The exception was that the subject was required to describe the remote location during a 15-min period beginning 20 min before the target was selected and 35 min before the outbound experimenter was to arrive at the target location. In detail, as shown in Table IX, each day at ten o'clock one of the experimenters would leave SRI with a stack of ten sealed envelopes from a larger pool and randomized daily, con- taining traveling instructions that had been prepared, but that were unknown to the two experimenters remaining with the subject. The subject for this experiment was Hella Hammid (S4) who participated in the nine-experiment series replicating the original Price work described earlier. The traveling experi- menter was to drive continuously from 10:00 until 10:30 be- fore selecting his destination with a random number generator. (The motivation for continuous motion was our observation that objects and persons in rapid motion are not generally seen in the remote-viewing mode of perception, and we wished the traveler to be a poor target until he reached his target site.) At the end of 30 min of driving, the traveling experimenter gener- TABLE IX EXPERIMENTAL PROTOCOL: PRECOGNIrivE REMOTE VIEWING Time Experimenter/SubjecL Activity ched.l. I I 10:00 Outbound experimenter leaves with 10 envelopes (containing target locations) and random number generator; begins half-hour drive 10:10 Experimenters remaini P-S th subject in the laboratory elicit from subject a dewsctription of where outbound experimenter will be from 10:45-11:00 10:25 Subject response completed, at which time laboratory part of experiment is over 10:30 Outbound experimenter obtairs random number from a random number generator, counts down to associated envelope, and proceeds to target locat@or indicated 10:45 Outbound experimenter remains at target location for 15 minutes (10:45-11:00) ated a random digit from 0 to 9 with a Texas Instruments SR-51 random number generator; while still in motion, he counted down that number of envelopes and proceeded di- rectly to the target location so as to arrive there by 10:45. He remained at the target site until 11:00, at which time he re- turned to the laboratory, showed his chosen target name to a security guard, and entered the experimental room. During the same period, the protocol in the laboratory was as follows. At 10: 10, the subject was asked to begin a descrip- tion of the place to which the experimenter would go 35 min hence. The subject then generated a tape-recorded description and associated drawings from 10:10 to 10:25, at which time her part in the experiment was ended. Her description was thus entirely concluded 5 min before the beginning of the tar- get selection procedure. Four such experiments were carried out. Each of them ap- peared to be successful, an evaluation later verified in blind judging without error by three judges. We will briefly sum- marize the four experiments below. The first target, the Palo Alto Yacht Harbor, consisted en- tirely of mud flats because of an extremely low tide (see Fig. 14). Appropriately, the entire transcript of the subject per- tained to "some kind of congealing tar, or maybe an area of condensed lava. It looks like the whole area is covered with some kind of wrinkled elephant skin that has oozed out to fill up some kind of boundaries where (the outbound experi- menter) is standing." Because of the lack of water, the dock where the remote experimenter was standing was in fact rest- ing directly on the mud. Approved For Release 2003/06/24 CIA P79-00999AO00300060005-1 Fig. 14. Subject Hammid (S4) described "some kind of congealing tar, or maybe an area of condensed lava ... that has oozed out to fill up some kind of boundaries." Approved For Release. 2003/06/24 348 Note that the subject has learned not to rush into interpreta- tion as to the nature or purpose of the place. This is a result of our cautioning based on the observation that such efforts tend to be purely analytical and in our experience are almost invariably incorrect. If a subject can limit himself to what he sees, he is often then able to describe a scene with sufficient accuracy that an observer can perform the analysis for him and identify the place. The second target visited was the fountain at one end of a large formal garden at Stanford University Hospital (Fig. 15). The subject gave a lengthy description of a formal garden be- hind a wall with a "double colonnade" and "very well mani- cured." When we later took the subject to the location, she was herself taken aback to find the double colonnaded wall leading into the garden just as described. The third target was a children's swing at a small park 4.6 km from the laboratory (Fig. 16). The subject repeated again and again that the main focus of attention at the site was a "black iron triangle that the outbound experimenter had somehow .walked into or was standing on." The triangle was "bigger than a man," and she heard a "squeak, squeak, about once a second," which we observe is a match to the black metal swing that did squeak. CIA-R 1976 Fig. 17. Subject (S4) described a very tall structure locatod among city streets and covered with "Tiffany-like glass.' The final target was the Palo Alto City Hall (Fig. 17). The subject described a very, very tall structure covered with "Tiffany-like glass." She had it located among city streets and with little cubes at the base. The building is glass-covered, and the little cubes are a good match to the small elevator exit buildings located in the plaza in front of the building. To obtain a numerical evaluation of the accuracy of the pre- cognitive viewing, the experimental results were subjected to independent judging on a blind basis by three SRI scientists who were not otherwise associated with the experiment. The judges were asked to match the four locations, which they visited, against the unedited typed manuscripts of the tape- recorded narratives, along with the drawings generated by the remote viewer. The transcripts were presented unlabeled and in random order and were to be used without replacement. A correct match required that the transcript of a given experi- ment be matched with the target of that experiment. All three judges independently matched the target data to the response data without error. Under the null hypothesis (no information channel and a random selection of descriptions without re- placement), each judge independently obtained a result signifi- cant at p = (41)_1 = 0.042. For reasons we do not as yet understand, the four transcripts generated in the precognition experiment show exceptional co- herence and accuracy as evidenced by the fact that all of the judges were able to match successfully all of the transcripts to Approved For Release@ 2003/06/24: CJA-RDR7c' "0999AO00300060005-1 Fig. 16. Subject (S4) saw a "black iron triangle that Hal had somehow walked into" and heard a "squeak, squeak, about once a %econd." Fig. I S. Subject (S4) described a formal garden "very well manicured" behind a double colonnade. PUTHOFF AND TARGApardw9d~FLctrHRRk%;tskgQP;fflW2A6p%7.~W-00999AO00300060005-1 349 the corresponding target locations. A long-range experimental program devoted to the clarification of these issues and involv- ing a number of subjects is under way. The above four experi- ments are the first four carried out under this program. Currently, we have no precise model of this spatial and tem- poral remote-viewing phenomenon. However, models of the universe involving higher order synchronicity or correlation have been proposed by the physicist Pauli and the psychologist Carl Jung [621. ACAUSALITY. If natural law5 were an absolute truth, then of course there could not possibly be any processes that deviate from it. But since causalitys is a statistical truth, it holds good only on average and thus leaves room for exceptions which must somehow be experienceable, that is to say, real. I try to regard synchronistic events as acausal exceptions of this kind. They prove to be relatively independent of space and time; they rela- tivize space and time insofar as space presents in principle no ob- stacle to their passage and the sequence of events in time is in- verted so that it looks as if an event which has not yet occurred were causing a perception in the present. We shall see in the next section that such a description, though poetic, has some basis in modern physical theory. V. DISCUSSION It is important to note at the outset that many contempo- rary physicists are of the view that the phenomena that we have been discussing are not at all inconsistent with the framework of physics as currently understood. In this emerg- ing view, the often-held belief that observations of this type are incompatible with known laws in principle is erroneous, such a concept being based on the naive realism prevalent before the development of modern quantum theory and information theory. One hypothesis, put forward by 1. M. Kogan of the USSR, is that information transfer under conditions of sensory shielding is mediated by extremely low-frequency (ELF) electromagnetic waves in the 300-1000-km region [371- [40]. Experimental support for the hypothesis is claimed on the basis of slower than inverse square attenuation, com- patible with source-percipient distances lying in the induc- tion field range as opposed to the radiation field range@ ob- served low bit rates (0.005-0.1 bit/s) compatible with the information carrying capacity of ELF waves; apparent ineffec- tiveness of ordinary electromagnetic shielding as an attenuator; and standard antenna calculations entailing biologically gener- ated currents yielding results compatible with observed signal- to-noise ratios. M. Persinger, Psychophysiology Laboratory, Laurentian Uni- versity, Toronto, Canada, has narrowed the ELF hypothesis to the suggestion that the 7.8-11z "Shumann waves" and their harmonics propagating along the earth-ionosphere waveguide duct may be responsible. Such an hypothesis is compatible with driving by brain-wave currents and leads to certain other hypotheses such as asymmetry between east-west and west- east propagation, preferred experimental times (midnight-4 A.m.), and expected negative correlation between success and the U index (a measure of geomagnetic disturbance throughout the world). Persinger claims initial support for these factors on the basis of a literature search [ 631, [ 641 . On the negative side with regard to a straightforward ELF interpretation as a blanket hypothesis are the following: a) ap- As usually understood. parent real-time descriptions of remote activities in sufficient detail to require a channel capacity in all probability greater than that allowed by a conventional modulation of an ELF signal; b) lack of a proposed mechanism for coding and decod- ing the information onto the proposed ELF carrier; and c) ap- parent precognition data. The hypothesis must nonetheless re- main open at this stage of research, since it is conceivable that counterindication a) may eventually be circumvented on the basis that the apparent high bit rate results from a mixture of low bit rate input and high bit rate "filling in the blanks" from imagination; counterindication b) is common to a number of normal perceptual tasks and may therefore simply reflect a lack of sophistication on our part with regard to perceptual functioning [651 ; and counterindication c) may be accom- modated by an ELF hypothesis if advanced waves as well as retarded waves are admitted [661, [67]. Experimentation to determine whether the ELF hypothesis is viable can be carried out by the use of ELF sources as targets, by the study of para- metric dependence on propagational directions and diurnal timing, and by the exploration of interference effects caused by creation of a high-intensity ELF environment during ex- perimentation, all of which are under consideration in our lab- oratory and elsewhere. Some physicists believe that the reconciliation of observed paranormal functioning with modern theory may take place at a more fundamental level-namely, at the level of the founda- tions of quantum theory. There is a continuing dialog, for example, on the proper interpretation of the effect of an ob- server (consciousness) on experimental measurement [681, and there is considerable current interest in the implications for our notions of ordering in time and space brought on by the observation [691, [701 of nonlocal correlation or "quan- tum interconnect edness" (to use Bohm's term [71 ] ) of distant parts of quantum systems of macroscopic dimensions. The latter, Bell's theorem [72], emphasizes that "no theory of reality compatible with quantum theory can require spatially separated events to be independent" [731, but must permit interconnectedness of distant events in a manner that is con- trary to ordinary experience [741-[751. This prediction has been experimentally tested and confirmed in the recent experiments of, for example, Freedman and Clauser [691, [ 701. E. H. Walker and 0. Costa de Beauregard, independently proposing theories of paranormal functioning based on quan- turn concepts, argue that observer effects open the door to the possibility of nontrivial coupling between consciousness and the environment and that the nonlocality principle permits such coupling to transcend spatial and temporal barriers [76], [771. Apparent "time reversibility"-that is, effects (e.g., observa- tions) apparently preceding causes (e.g., events)-though con- ceptually difficult at first glance, may be the easiest of appar- ent paranormal phenomena to assimilate within the current theoretical structure of our world view. In addition to the familiar retarded potential solutions f(t - r/0, it is well known that the equations of, for example, the electromagnetic field admit of advanced potential solutions f(t + r/c)-solutions that would appear to imply a reversal of cause and effect. Such solutions are conventionally discarded as not corresponding to any observable physical event. One is cautioned, however, by statements such as that of Stratton in his basic text on electro- magnetic theory [ 7 8 1. Approved For Release 2003/06/24 - Cl',A-RDFt79-00999AO00300060005-1 350 Approved For Release 2003/06/24. The reader has doubtless noted that the choice of the functi,)n f(t - r1c) is highly arbitrary, since the field equation admits aio a solution fit + r1c). This function leads obviously to an advanced time, implying that the field can be observed before it has been generated by the source. The familiar chain of cause and effect is thus reversed and this alternative solution might be discarded as logically inconceivable. However, the application of "logical" causality principles offers very insecure footing in matters such as these and we shall do better to restrict the theory to retarded action solely on the grounds that this solution alone conforms to the present physical data. Such caution is justified by the example in the early 1920's of Dirac's development of the mathematical description of the 'telativistic electron that also yielded a pair of solutions, one of which was discarded as inapplicable until the discovery of the positron in 1932. In an analysis by 0. Costa de Beauregard, an argument is put forward that advanced potentials constitute a convergence toward "finality" in a manner symmetrical to the divergence of retarded potentials as a result of causality [771. Such phenomena are generally unobservable, however, on the gross macroscopic scale for statistical reasons. This is codified in the thermodynamic concept that for an isolated system entropy (disorder) on the average increases. It is just this requirement of isolation, however, that has been weakened by the observer problem in quantum theory, and 0. Costa de Beauregard argues that the finality principle is maximally operative in just those situations where the intrusion of consciousness as an ordering phenomenon results in a significant local reversal of entropy increase. At this point, further discussion of the subtleties of such considerations, though apropos, would take us far afield, so we simply note that such advanced waves, if detected, could in certain cases constitute a carrier of information precognitive to the event. The above arguments are not intended to indicate that the precise nature of the information channel coupling @mote events and human perception is understood. Raher, we in- tend to show only that modern theory is not without resources that can be brought to bear on the problems at hand, and we expec* that these problems will, with further work, continue to yield to analysis and specification Furthermore, independent of the mechanisms that may be involved in remote sensing, observation of the phenomenon implies the existence of an information channel in the inform a tion-the ore tic sense. Since such channels are amenable to analysis on the basis of communication theory techniques ' as indicated earlier, channel characteristics such as bit rate can be determined independent of a well-defined physical channel model in the sense that thermodynamic concepts can be ap- plied to the analysis of systems independent of underlying mechanisms. Furthermore, as we have seen from the work of RyzI discussed in Section II, it is possible to use such a channel for error-free transmission of information ifredundancy coding is used. (See also Appendix A.) Therefore, experimentation involving the collection of data under specified conditions per- mits headway to be made despite the formidable work that needs to bd done to clarify the underlying bases of the phenomena. V1. CONCLUSION For the past three years we have had a program in the Elec- tronics and Biciengineering Laboratory of SRI to invcstigate those facets of human perception that appear to fall outside the range of well-understood perceptual or processing capa- CIA-RDIP797009WPAP9,9~9~)Q~OP~,--IEi~F, MARCH 19?t, bilities. The primary achievement of this program has been the elicitation of high-quality "remote viewing" the ability of both experienced subjects and inexperienced volunteers to view, by means of innate mental processes, remote geo- graphical or technical targets such as roads, buildings, and laboratory apparatus. Our accumulated data from over fifty experiments with more than a half-dozen subjects indicate the following. a) The phenomenon is not a sensitive function of distance over I range of several kilometers. b) Faradwy cage shielding does not appear to degrade the quality or accuracy oi perception. c) Most of the correct information that subject,,, relate is of a nonanalytic nature pertaining to shape. form, color, and material rather than to function or name (Thi,, aspect suggests a hypothesis that information trans in issio r under conditions of sensory shielding ma@ @'e mediated pri' marily by the brain's right hemisphere ) d) The principal difference between experienced subjects and inexperienced volunteers is not that the latter never exhibit the faculty, but rather that their results are simply less reliable. (This ,hserva- tion suggests the hypothesis that remote viewing may be a latent and widely distributed, though repressed, pe7ceptual ability.) Although the precise nature ofthe information -hannel cou- pling remote events and human PerCePtiOll is not YCI Undei stood, certain concepts in information theory, quaritum theory, and neufophysiological research appear to bear dlrectl@ on the issue. As a result, the working assaimption among re- searchers in the field is that the phenomenon of intervsi i@ consistent with modern scientific thought, and (an tht,reion be expected to yield to the scientific methc)(J. F@urt@,,o@, ;, k recognized thw communication tneor: provides i )- tz@rtul techniques, such as the use of redu.iddn,-y codin.,, to mprove signal-to-noise ratio, which can be eniploycJ to pursue special purpose application of the remote-sensingcILannel indeoenden, of an understanding of the underlying mechanisn,s. W" 1herc- fore consider it important to continu,_@ data collection and t(; encourage others to do likewise: investigations such @,,, those reported here need replication and extension under a,- wide @i variety of rigorously controlled conditions as possible. APPENDix A SIGNAL LNHANCEMENJ IN A PAl-,ANOPMAI. COMMUNICATION CHANtJEI By APPLACATIO"@ OF REDUNDANCY CODING Independent of the mechanisms that may he involveci in remote sensing, observation of the phenomenon iraplies th@: existence of ao information channel in the inforruation- theoretic sense As we have seen from thc work of kyzl dis- cussed in Section ll,5 it is even possible to use such a (noisy) channel for error-free transmission of information if sut- ficient redundancy coding is used [30], f311. Folloi,ving is a general procedure that we have used successfully f@-.r signal enhancement. We shall assunie that the "message" consists of a stTearil o,l binary digits (0,I) of equal probability (e.g., binary sort of green/white cards as in Ryzl's case. English text encoded as it, Table X and sent long distance by strobe light on/off, and so on). To combat channel noise, each binary digit to be sent through the channel requires the addition of redundancy bits (coding). Efficient coding requires a compromise bet@,veen the desire to maximize reliability ind the desire to min,tilize rc 6See also the note added in proof oii the succ-sful worl@ (lone 1,;. Carpenter. Approved For Release 2003/06/24: CIA-RDP79-00999AO00300060005-1 PUTHOFF AND TARApprwve~diFiiDr-i-kWeaser(206&OSWAtcOlARRW70-00999AO00300060005-1 TABLE X 5-BIT CODE FOR ALPHANUMERIC CHARACTERS E 00000 Y 01000 T 11111 G,J 10111 N 00001 W 01001 R 11110 v 10110 1 00010 B 01010 0 11101 0 10101 A 00011 1 01011 S,X,Z 11100 2 10100 D 00100 3 01100 H 11011 4 10011 L 00101 5 01101 C,K,Q 11010 6 10010 F 00110 7 01110 P 11001 8 10001 U 00111 9 01111 M 11000 10000 Note: Alphabet characters listed in order of decreasing frequency in English text. See, for example, A. Sinkov [79]. (The low-fre- cluency letters, X, Z, K, Q, and J, have been grouped with similar characters to provide space for numerics in a 5-bit code.) In consideration of the uneven dis- tribution of letter frequencies in English text, this code is chosen such that 0 and 1 have equal probability. dundancy. One efficient coding scheme for such a channel is obtained by application of a sequential sampling procedure of the type used in production-line quality control [801. The adaptation of such a procedure to paranormal communication channels, which we now discuss, was considered first by Taetzsch [ 8 1 ] . The sequential method gives a rule of proce- dure for making one of three possible decisions following the receipt of each bit: accept I as the bit being transmitted ; reject I as the bit being transmitted (i.e., accept 0); or continue transmission of the bit under consideration. The sequential sampling procedure differs from fixed-length coding in that the number of bits required to reach a final decision on a message bit is not fixed before transmission, but depends on the results accumulated with each transmission. The principal advantage of the sequential sampling procedure as compared with the other methods is that, on the average, fewer bits per final decision are required for an equivalent degree of reliability. Use of the sequential sampling procedure requires the speci- fication of parameters that are determined on the basis of the following considerations. Assume that a message bit (0 or 1) is being transmitted. In the absence of a priori knowledge, we may assume equal probability (p = 0.5) for the two possibili- ties (0, 1 ). Therefore, from the standpoint of the receiver, the probability of correctly identifying the bit being transmitted is p = 0.5 because of chance alone. An operative remote-sensing channel could then be expected to alter the probability of correct identification to a value p = 0.5 + 0, where the param- eter @ satisfies 0 < I V1 I < 0.5. (The quantity may be positive or negative depending on whether the paranormal channel results in so-called psi-hitting or psi-missing.) Good psi func- tioning on a repetitive task has been observed to result in ip = 0.12, as reported by RyzI [311. Therefore, to indicate the design procedure, let us assume a baseline psi parameter tPb = 0. 1 and design a communication system on this basis. z < 50 < z V)LU 40 L)uj @- V) < 2 (LD' 30 U. < 0 @2 cc LU Lu2 20 M 2 =) LLJ Z 0 X 10 UJ < > F- < - 0 _J' D 0 351 DECISION 1 Accept "1" as - @ the Bit Being _ Transmitted ECISION 3,-- C."tinue - ransr i-sion D ECISION 2 Accept "0" as the Bit Being Transmitted 1 1 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 NUMBER OF TRIALS Fig. 18. Enhancement of signal-to-noise ratio by sequential sampling procedure (p. = 0.4, p, = 0.6, a = 0.01, 0 = 0.01). The question to be addressed is whether, after repeated transmission, a given message bit is labeled a "I" at a low rate po commensurate with the hypothesis Ho that the bit in ques- tion is a "0," or at a higher rate p, commensurate with the hypothesis H] that the bit in question is indeed a "I." The decision-making process requires the specification of four parameters. po The probability of labeling incorrectly a "0" message bit as a " l." The probability of labeling correctly a "0" as a "0" is P = 0-5 + Ob = 0.6. Therefore, the probability of labeling incorrectly a "0 " as a "I" is 1 - p = 0.4 = p 0. p, The probability of labeling correctly a "I" message bit as a "I," is given by p, = 0.5 + Ob = 0.6. Ce The probability of rejecting a correct identification for a "0" (Type I error). We shall take cit = 0.01. The probability of accepting an incorrect identification for a "I" (Type 11 error). We shall take 0.01. With the parameters thus specified, the sequential sampling procedure provides for construction of a decision graph as shown in Fig. 18. The equations for the upper and lower limit lines are =d, +SN I 0 -do + SN where 1 Ce log log d, =- do = -- log pi- I - Po log Pi po Po I - PI Po Pi I Po log S - log pi I - Po Po 1 - p I in which S is the slope, N is the number of trials, and d, and do are the y-axis intercepts. A cumulative record of receiver- generated responses to the target bit is compiled until either Approved For Release 2003/06/24: CIA-RDP79-00999AO00300060005-1 352 Approved For Release 2003/06/24: C1A-RDR7b-009NA000M0WQWitEE, MARCH 1976 1.0 -z:; @0E 0.8 C 2 >0) C - 0.6 1B M -C 0tm 0.4 E J 0.2 < _J LU CD Cr _0 +0.4 +0.2 0 -0.2 -0.4 0 (psi parameter) viewing. The target, determined by random procedure, was White's Plaza, a plaza with fountain at Stanford University (shown in Fig. 8). As is our standard protocol, the experi- menter with the subject is kept ignorant of the specific target visited as well as the contents of the target pool. The experi- menter's statements and questions are italics. Today is Monday, October 7th. It is I L-00 and this is a re- mote viewing experiment with Russ Targ, Phyllis Cole, and Hal Puthoff In this experiment Hal will drive to a remote site chosen by a random process. Phyllis Cole will be the remote viewer, and Russ Targ is the monitor. We expect this experi- ment to start at twenty minutes after eleven and run for fif- teen minutes. It is just about twenty minutes after eleven and Hal should be at his target location by now. Why don't you tell me what kind of pictures you see and what you think he might be doing or experiencing. The first thing that came to mind was some sort of a large, square kind of a shape. Like Hal was in front of it. It was a ... not a building or something, it was a square. I don't know if it was a window, but something like that so that the bottom line of it was not at the ground. About where his waist was, at least. That's what it seemed to me. It seems outdoors some- how. Tree. Does Hal seem to be looking at that square? I don't know. The first impression was that he wasn't, but I have a sense that whatever it was was something one might look at. I don't know if it would be a sign, but something that one might look at. Can you tell if it is on the ground or vertical? It seemed vertical. I don't have a sense that it was part of anything particular. It might be on a building or part of a building, but I don't know. There was a tree outside, but I also got the impression of cement. I don't have the impression of very many people or traffic either. I have the sense that he is sort of walking back and forth. I don't have any more explicit picture than that. Can you move into where he is standing and try to see what he is looking at? I picked up he was touching something-- something rough. Maybe warm and rough. Something possibly like cement It is twenty-four minutes after eleven. Can you change your point of view and move above the scene so you can get a bigger picture of what's there? I still see some trees and some sort of pavement or some- thing like that. Might be a courtyard. The thing that came to mind was it might be one of the plazas at Stanford campus or something like that, cement. Some kinds of landscaping. I said Stanford campus when I started to see some things in White Plaza, but I think that is misleading. I have the sense that he's not moving around too much. That it's in a small area. I guess I'll go ahead and say it, but I'm afraid I'm just putting on my impressions from Stanford campus. I had the impres- sion of a fountain. There are two in the plaza, and it seemed that Hal 'was possibly near the, what they call Mern Claw. What is that? It's a fountain that looks rather like a claw. It's a black sculpture. And it has benches around it made of cement. Are there any buildings at the place you are looking at? Are there any buildings? You described a kind of a courtyard. Fig. 19. Reliability curve for sequential sampling procedure (p. 0.4, P, = 0.6, a = 0.01, 9 = 0.01). the upper or the lower limit line is reached, at which point a decision is made to accept 0 or I as the bit being transmitted. Channel reliability (probability of correctly determining message being transmitted) as a function of operative psi parameter ip is plotted in Fig. 19. As observed, the sequential sampling procedure can result in 90 percent or greater reliability with psi parameters on the order of a few percent. Implementation of the sequential sampling procedure re- quires the transmission of a message coded in binary digits. Therefore, the target space must consist of dichotomous ele- ments such as the white and green cards used in the experi- ments by Ryzl. In operation, a sequence corresponding to the target bit (0 or I ) is sent and the cumulative entries are made (Fig. 18) until a decision is reached to accept either a 1 or a 0 as the bit being transmitted. At a prearranged time, the next sequence is begun and continues as above until the entire message has been received. A useful alternative, which relieves the percipient of the burden of being aware of his self-contradiction from trial to trial, consists of cycling through the entire message repeti- tively and entering each response on its associated graph until a decision has been reached on all message bits. The authors have used this technique successfully in a pilot study, but a discussion of this would take us beyond the intended scope of this paper. From the results obtained in such experiments, the channel bit rate can be ascertained for the system configuration under consideration. Furthermore, bit rates for other degree, of reliability (i.e., for other po, p, , a, and 0) can be estimated by construction of other decision curves over the same data base and thus provide a measure of the bit rate per degree of reliability. In summary, the procedures described here can provide for a specification of the characteristics of a remote-sensing channel under well-defined conditions. These procedures also provide for a determination of the feasibility of such a channel for particular applications. APPENDix B REMOTE-VIEWING TRANSCRIPT Following is the unedited transcript of the first experiment with an SRI volunteer (S6), a mathematician in the computer science laboratory, with no previous experience in remote Approved For Release 2003/06/,@4 WRDP79-@00999AO00300060005-1 @ @, a @',_ fL, Approved For Release-100106124: CIA-RDP-7~'9;00999AO00300060005-1 PUTHOFF AND TARG: PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TkAN§ffR' Usually at some places there should be a building, large or small that the courtyard is about. Look at the end or the sides ol the courtyard. Is there anything to be seen? I have a sense that there are buildings. It's not solid build- ings. I mean there are some around the periphery and I have a sense that none of them are very tall. Maybe mostly one story, maybe an occasional two story one. Do you have any better idea of what your square was that you saw at the outset? No. I could hazard different kinds of guesses. Does it seem part of this scene? It ... I think it could be. It could almost be a bulletin board or something with notices on it maybe. Or something that people are expected to look at. Maybe a window with things in it that people were expected to look at. What kind of trees do you see in this place? I don't know what kind they are. The impression was that they were shade trees and not terribly big. Maybe 12 feet of trunk and then a certain amount of branches above that. So that the branches have maybe a 12 foot diameter, or some- thing. Not real big trees. New trees rather than old trees? Yeah, maybe 5 or 10 years old, but not real old ones. Is there anything interesting about the pavement? No. It seems to be not terribly new or terribly old. Not very interesting. There seems to be some bits of landscaping around. Little patches of grass around the edges and periph- eries. Maybe some flowers. But, not lush. You saw some benches. Doyou want to tell me about them? Well, that's my unsure feeling about this fountain. There was some kind of benches of cement. Curved benches, it felt like. They were of rough cement. What do you think Hal is doing while he is there? 1 have a sense that he is looking at things trying to project them. Looking at different things and sort of walking back and forth not covering a whole lot of territory. Sometimes standing still while he looks around. I just had the impression of him talking, and I almost sense that it was being recorded or something. I don't know if he has a tape recorder, but if it's not that, then he is saying some- thing because it needed to be remembered. It's 11:33. He's just probably getting ready to come back. ACKNOWLEDGMENT The authors wish to thank the principal subjects, Mrs. Hella Hammid, Pat Price, and Ingo Swann, who showed patience and forbearance in addition to their enthusiasm and outstanding perceptual abilities. We note with sadness the death of one of our subjects, MT. Price. We express our sincere thanks also to Earle Jones, Bonnar Cox, and Dr. Arthur Hastings, of SRI, and Mrs. Judith Skutch and Richard Bach, without whose en- couragement and support this work could not have taken place. REFERENCES [11 J. R. Smythies, Ed., Science and ESP. London, England: Rout- ledge, 1967. 121 C. Evans, "Parapsychology -What the questionnaire revealed," New Scientist, Jan. 25, 1973, p. 209. 131 A. Gauld, The Founders of Psychical Research. New York: Schocken Books, 1968. See also W. Crookes, Researches in the Phenomena ofSpiritualism. London, England: J. Burns, 1874. 141 R. Targ and H. Puthoff, "Information transmission under condi- tions of sensory shielding," Nature, vol. 252, pp. 602-607, Oct. 353 18,1974. [51 D. D. Home, Lights and Shadows of Spiritualism. New York: G. W. Carleton, 1877. [61 J. Coover, Experiments in Psychical Research. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford Univ. Press, 1917. [71 G. Estabrooks, Bull. Boston Society for Psychical Research, 1927. See also [ 12, pp. 18 -19 1. [81 L. T. Troland, Techniques for the Experimental Study of Telep- athy and OtherAlleged Clairvoyant Processes. Albany, NY, 1928. [91 J. B. Rhine, New Frontiers of the Mind. New York: Farrar and Rinehart, 1937. 1101 J. Pratt and J. B. Rhine et at., Extra-Sensory Perception after Sixty Years. New York: Henry Holt, 1940. 1111 C. Scott, "G. Spencer Brown and probability; A critique," J. Soc. PsychicalRes., vol. 39, pp. 217-234, 1958. 1121 G. R. Price, "Science and the supernatural," Science, vol. 122, pp.359-367,1955. [131 -, "Apology to Rhine and Soal," Science, vol. 175, p. 359@ 1972. 1141 J. B. Rhine, "A new case of experimenter unreliability," J. Parapsychol., vol. 38, pp. 215-225, June 1974. [151 S. G. Soal and F Bateman, Modern Experiments in Telepathy. London, England: Faber and Faber, 1953. [161 C Scott and P. Haskell, "'Normal' explanation of the Soal- G'oldney experiments in extra-sensory perception," Nature, vol. 245, pp. 52-54, Sept. 7, 1973. [171 C@ E. M. Hansel, ESP-A Scientific Evaluation. New York: Scribner, 1966. 1181 J. B. Rhine and J. G. Pratt, "A review of the Pearce-Pratt dis- tance series of ESP tests," J. Parapsychol., vol. 18, pp. 165-177, 1954. [19] J. G. Pratt and J. L. Woodruff, "Size of stimulus symbols in extra- sensory perception," J. Parapsychol., vol. 3, pp. 121-158, 1939. [20] S. G. Soal and H. T. Bowden, The Mind Readers: Recent Experi- mentsin Telepathy. New Haven, CT: Yale Univ. Press, 1954. [21] C. Honorton, "Error some place!" J. Commun., vol. 25, no. I (Annenberg School of Commun.), Winter 197 5. [221 M. Ryzl, "Training the psi faculty by hypnosis," J. Amer. Soc. PsychicalRes., vol. 41, pp. 234-251, 1962. [231 CIBA Foundation Symposium on Extra Sensory Perception. Boston, MA: Little, Brown, 1956. 1241 M. RyzI and J. Pratt, "A repeated-calling ESP test with sealed cards," J. Parapsychol., vol. 27, pp. 161-174, 1963. [25] -, "A further confirmation of stabilized ESP performance in a selected subject," J. Parapsychol., vol. 27, pp. 73-83, 1963. [261 J. Pratt, "Preliminary experiments with a 'borrowed' ESP sub- ject," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 42, pp. 333-345, 1964. 1271 J. Pratt and 1. Blom, "A confirmatory experiment with 'bor- rowed' outstanding ESP subject," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 42, pp. 391-398, 1964. [28] W. G. Roll and J. G. Pratt, "An ESP test with aluminum targets," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 62, pp. 381-387, 1968. [291 J. Pratt, "A decade of research with a selected ESP subject: An overview and reappraisal of the work with Pavel Stepanek," Proc. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 30, 1973. [301 C. Shannon and W. Weaver, The Mathematical Theory of Com- munication. Urbana, IL: Univ. Illinois Press, 1949. 131] M. Ryzl, "A model for parapsychological communication," J. Parapsychol., vol. 30, pp. IS -3 1, Mar. 1966. [321 C. Tart, "Card guessing tests: Learning paradigm or extinction paradigm," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 60, p. 46, 1966. 1331 M. Ullman and S. Krippner, with A. Vaughan, Dream Telepathy. New York: Macmillan, 1973. 1341 C. Honorton, "State of awareness factors in psi activation," J. Amer. Soc, Psychical Res., vol. 68, pp. 246-257, 1974@ (351 Proc. 2nd Int. Congr. Psychotronic Research (Monte Carlo). Cotati, CA: Int. Assoc. Psychotronic Res., 1975. [361 L. L. Vasiliev, Experiments in Mental Suggestion. Hampshire, England: ISMI Publ., 1963. [37] 1. M. Kogan, "is telepathy possible?" Radio Eng., vol. 2 1, p. 75, Jan. 1966. [381 -, "Telepathy, hypotheses and observations," Radio Eng., vol. 22, p. 141, Jan. 1967. [39] -, "Information theory analysis of telepathic communication experiments," Radio Eng., vol. 2 3, p. 122, Mar. 19 68. [401 -, "The information theory aspect of telepathy," RAND Publ., Santa Monica, CA, p. 4145, July 1969. [411 A. S. Presman, Electromagnetic Fields and Life. New York: Plenum, 1970. [421 Y. A. Kholodov, Ed., Influence of Magnetic Fields on Biological Objects, JPRS 63038, NTIS, Springfield, VA, Sept. 24, 1974. [431 Y. A. Kholodov, "Investigation of the direct effect of magnetic fields on the central nervous system," in Proc. Ist Conf. Psycho- ironic Res., JPRS L15022-1 and 2, Sept. 6, 1974. [44] D. Mennie, "Consumer electronics," IEEE Spectrum, vol. 12, pp. 34-35, Mar. 1975. [451 W. P. Zinchenko, A. N. Leontiev, B. M. Lornov, and A. R. Luria, Approved For Release 2003/06/24: CIA-RDP79-00999AO00300060005-1 354 [461 [471 (481 (491 [501 1511 521 531 1,54] 155 J 1561 1571 1581 1591 1601 1611 1621 1631 Approved For Release 2003/06/24: CIA-~~6~,,,79-00999AO00300060005-1 @ I - @-' ;f PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 "Parapsychology: Fiction or reality?" Questions ofPhilosophy, vol. 9, pp. 128-136, 1973. R. Cavanna, Ed., Proc. Int. Conf. Methodology in PSI Research. New York: Parapsychology Foundation, 1970. E. D. Dean, "Plethysmograph recordings as ESP responses," Int. J. Neuropsychiatry, vol. 2, Sept. 1966. C. Tart, "Physiological correlates of psi cognition," Int. J. Para- psychol., no. 4, 1963. D. H. Lloyd, "Objective events in the brain correlating with psychic phenomena," New Horizons, vol. 1, no. 2, Summer 1973. J. Silverman and M. S. Buchsbaum, "Perceptual correlates of con- sciousness; A conceptual model and its technical implications for psi research," in Psi Favorable States of Consciousness, R. Ca- vanna, Ed. New York: Parapsychology Foundation, pp. 143- 169, 1970. J. Kamiya, "Comment to Silverman and Buchsbaum," ibid., pp. 158-159. D. Hill, and G. Parr, Electroencephalography. A Symposium on Its Various Aspects. New York: Macmillan, 1963. T. D. Duane and T. Behrendt, "Extrasensory electroencephalo- graphic induction between identical twins," Science, vol. 150, p.367,1965. K. Osis, ASPR Newsletter, no. 14, 1972. R. L. Morris, "An exact method for evaluating preferentially matched free-response material," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 66, p. 40 1, Oct. 1972. G. R. Schmeidler, "PK effects upon continuously recorded tem- peratures," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 67, no. 4, Oct. 1973. W. Scherer, "Spontaneity as a factor in ESP," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 12, pp. 12 6-147, 1948. R. Targ, P. Cole, and H. Puthoff, "Techniques to enhance man/ machine communication," SRI, Menlo Park, CA, Final Rep., NASA Contract NAS7-1 00, June 1974. R. Ornstein, The Nature of Human Consciousness. San Fran- cisco, CA: Freeman, 1973, ch. 7 and 8. R. W. Sperry, "Cerebral organization and behavior," Science, vol. 133, pp. 1749-1757, 1961. 0. Bilaniuk and E. C. G. Sudarshan, "Particles beyond the light barrier," Phys. Today, vol. 22, May 5, 1969. W. Pauli and C. G. Jung, Eds., The Interpretation of Nature and the Psyche (Bollingen Set. LI). Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press, 1955. M. A. Persinger, "ELF waves and ESP," New Horizons Trans. Toronto Society for Psychical Research, vol. 1, no. 5, Jan. 1975. [641 -, "The paranormal-P. 11: Mechanisms and models," M.S.S. Information Corp., New York, 1974. [651 B. Julesz, Foundations of Cyclopean Perception. Chicago, IL: Univ. Chicago Press, 197 1. 1661 H. Puthoff and R. Targ, in Psychic Exployation-A Challenge for Science, J. White, Ed. New York: Putnam, 1974, pp. 522-542. [67 G. Feinberg, "Precognition -A memory of things future?" in Proc. Conf. Quantum Physics and Parapsychology (Geneva, Switzerland). New York: Parapsychology Foundation, 1975. [68 E. P. Wigner, "The problem of measurement," Amer. J. Phys., vol. 31, no. 1, p. 6, 1963. [691 J. J. Freedman and J. F. Clauser, "Experimental test of local hidden variable theories," Phys. Rev. Lett., vol. 28, no. 14, p. 938, Apr. 3, 1972. 1701 J. F. Clauser and M. A. Horne, "Experimental consequences of objective local theories," Phys. Rev. D, vol. 10, no. 2, p. 526, July 15, 1974. J 711 D. Bohm and B. Hiley, "On the intuitive understanding of non- locality as implied by quantum theory" (Birkbeck College, London, England), Feb. 1974, Preprint. [721 J. S. Rell, "On the problem of hidden variables in quantum theory," Rev. Mod. Phys., vol. 38, no. 3, p. 447, July 1966. [731 H. Stapp, "Theory of reality," Lawrence-Berkeley Lab. Rep. LBL-3837, Univ. California, Berkeley, Apr. 1975. [741 A. Einstein, B. Podolsky, and N. Rosen, "Can quantum-mecham- cal description of physical reality be considered complete?" Phys. Rev., vol. 47, p. 777, May 15, 1935. [75 ] R. H. Dicke and J. P. Wittke, Introduction to Quantum Mechan- ics. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, 1960, ch. 7. [76] E. H. Walker, "Foundations of paraphysical and parapsychologi- cal phenomena," in Proc. Conf. Quantum Physics and Para- psychology (Geneva, Switzerland). New York: Parapsychology Foundation, 1975. [771 0. Costa de Beauregard, "Time symmetry and interpretation of quantum mechanics," Lecture delivered at Boston Colloquim for Philosophy of Science (Feb. 1974), Foundations ofPhysics (in press). [781 J. A. Stratton, Electromagnetic Theory. New York: McGraw- Hill, 1941. [791 A. Sinkov, Elementary Cryptanalysis-A Mathematical Approach. New York: Random House, 1968. 80) P. Hoel, Introduction to Mathematical Statistics, 2nd ed. New York: Wiley, 1954, p. 27. 811 R. Taetzsch, "Design of a psi communications system," Int. J. Parapsychol., vol. 4, no. 1, p. 35, Winter 1962. Approved For ReleaL§e,200.3./0,6/24:,C,,IA-P-69-00999AO00300060005-1 Approved For Release 20(13/O6/Z4,@ CIA-RDP79-00999AO00300060005-1 ILU Approved For Rehias4;A6 k-R P79-00999AO00300060005-1 PD. rIlDP79-00999AO00300060005-1 Approved For Release 2003/0abok Approved For Release 200&WAEII-RDP79-00999AO00300060005-1