Approved For Release 20 SECR 96-00788RO01 300070003-9 23 March 1979 PROPOSED GRILL FLAME PROTOCOL (U) PROPOSED SRI INTERNATIONAL APPLIED REMOTE VIEWING PROTOCOL (S) SRI 9-4107 Copy No. SECRET Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 '111111IMp proved For Release 2003/g,9LJO - P96-00788RO01 300070003-9 C filf cu 23 March 1979 PROPOSED GRILL FLAME PROTOCOL (U) im PROPOSED SRI INTERNATIONAL APPLIED REMOTE VIEWING PROTOCOL (S) h I ON 11 CLASSIFIED BY: Msg. HQDA(DAMI-ISH) Wash. REVIEW ON: 23 March 1999 NATIONAL SECURITY INFORMATION Unauthorized Disclosure Subject to Administrative and Criminal Sanctions q'tz Mal. MEN (tlnterna@fiona)l WARNING NOTICE - Intelligence SOUrces A and Methods Involved an E NOT RMAVAPLE TO FOREIGN FATIOMALS1 SRI 9-4107 This document consists of '.@ pages. Copy No. 17 of 15 copies. 333 Ravenswood Ave. Menlo Park, California 94025 (415) 326-6200 * Cable: STANRES, Menlo Park - TWX: 910-373-1246 Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 SECRET Approved For Release 2003/Q91A P96-00788RO01 300070003-9 t ktT M (S) PROPOSED GRILL FLAME PROTOCOL (U) (S) PROPOSED SRI INTERNATIONAL APPLIED REMOTE VIEWING PROTOCOL (S) 1. (S) GENERAL This protocol contains the procedure for AMSAA-sponsored SRI Inter- national-performed remote viewing. It is in effect for the period required to accomplish the scope of work. Remote viewing (RV) is an intellectual process by which a person perceives characteristics of a location remote from that person. RV does not involve any electronic sensing devices at or focused at the target site, nor does it involve classical photo inter- pretation of photographs obtained from overhead or oblique means. The individual performing RV (the remote viewer) is provided with a unique identifier such as stationary map coordinates, a specific structure, an identifiable vehicle (aircraft tail number) or a specific individual (name, place of birth, age, and/or photograph). The task of the remote viewer is to locate, identify and/or describe the target. The task is achievable.1-5 (Reference 1 is included here as Tab A.) No drugs, hypnosis, special sensory (visual, auditory or olfactory) or proprio- ceptive stimuli, liminal or subliminal, electrical or electromagnetic stimuli will be used in this RV protocol. 2. (S) MILITARY OBJECTIVE It is the objective of this protocol to familiarize Army and contractor personnel with RV procedures as they apply to the location and identifica- Hi tion of militarily significant targets. 3. (S) MILITARY APPLICATIONS RV provides a potential capability to target field-mobile weapons which are currently difficult or impossible to detect prior to launch, such as tactical missiles and rockets and attack helicopters. RV can be used to: target on key enemy military individuals, from covert agents to key battle commanders; detect the change in state of military units; to rapidly determine the damage resulting from nonnuclear weapon attack; to determine the access code to computers and other electronic devices; and ow to determine the general content of documents and other informational items found in military organizations. 1 SECRET Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 Approved For Release 2003STC: ff itIDP96-00788RO01 300070003-9 4. (S) APPROVAL HISTORY SRI International was requested by the U.S. Army Materiel Systems Analysis Activity (AMSAA) to prepare a proposal for an extension of their (SRI) previous RV work which would focus on familiarizing some Army personnel with RV procedures and application of existing RV procedures to tactical military targets. The Commander, U.S. Army Materiel Development and Readiness Command (DARCOM) approved in principle the U.S. Army Materiel Systems Analysis Activity (AMSAA) involvement in what is now known as project GRILL FLAME in April 1978. In May 1978, the Assistant Chief of Staff for Intelligence (ACSI) accepted lead responsibilty for GRILL FLAME applications. Overall DoD responsibility resides with the Defense Intelligence Agency (DIA). 5. (S) PROJECT'OFFICERS The overall, responsible individuals for all aspects of the project are Harold E. Puthoff, Ph.D. and Russell Targ (for curriculum vitae see Tabs B and C). 6. (S) DEFINITIONS a. Remote Viewing (RV): An intellectual process by which a person perceives characteristics of a location remote from that person; it does not involve any electronic sensing devices at or focused at the target nor does it involve classical photo interpretation of photographs obtained from overhead or oblique means. b. Remote Viewer: The person who locates, identifies and/or des- cribes the target. C. Interviewer: The person who interacts with the remote viewer before, during and after the RV session. d. Remote Viewing Session: A single attempt by the remote viewer to locate, identify and/or describe a target. 7. (S) PROCEDURE To provide a framework for familiarizing Army and contractor per- sonnel with the task of RV, a series of RV sessions will be conducted. 2 SECRET Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 Approved For Release 2003 ff c. nr P96-00788RO01 300070003-9 (S) The elements of an RV procedure are: (1) target selection; (2) remote viewer session preliminaries; (3) remote viewing session; and (4) post- session analysis. The procedure will now be described, using geographic coordinates as the remote target identifier. (1) TARGET SELECTION From a target pool of 50 to 100 geographical coordinates previously selected by an individual, called the target pool selector (TPS), the TPS will select a iarget for the session. This person does Nd not communicate at any time with the remote viewer or the interviewer. The 50 to 100 individual targets are randomized, numbered and stored in a secure container accessible only to the TPS. Targets once used, are not returned to the target pool. (2) REMOTE VIEWER SESSION PRELIMINARIES Before a first RV session is scheduled, the remote viewer is oriented by the interviewer to the procedure to be followed. The remote viewer needs to understand that he or she should.give initial raw perceptions; experience has shown' that later more deliberate interpre- tations are quite often wrong while the initial raw perception tends to be correct. Remote viewers are always encouraged to express their feelings and ideas for enhancing all aspects of the RV process. (3) REMOTE VIEWING SESSION During the 30 to 60 minutes prior to the agreed-upon start time of a session, the interviewer offers some encouragement to the remote viewer in the manner of a coach giving a pep talk to his team, and answers any questions the remote viewer may have about the procedures. During the 15 minutes immediately before the session, the remote viewer and interviewer are generally silent. Experience has shown (unpublished data) that this ,'quiet time" enhances the RV process. During the 15 minute viewing period the remote viewer and the interviewer function as a team. At no time is the session conducted by the remote viewer in the absence of all other persons. The inter- viewer provides encouragement with words of reassurance that the task is 3 SECRET Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 aw Approved For Release 20039t1t:kVDP96-OO788 ROO 1300070003-9 (S) MW in fact possible. Thus, if the remote viewer does not have any immediate sensory images, the interviewer applies no pressure; rather, the interviewer no reassures the remote viewer that they have all the time in the world. When the remote viewer has an image of the target site, the interviewer, in conversation with the remote viewer, may then suggest that the remote so viewer intellectually move around at the site and describe the site more fully (e.g., buildings, terrain features, people, activities, machinery, etc.). If it appears to the interviewer that the images are in some way contradictory or inconsistent, the interviewer may then attempt ON clarification by asking questions in order to verify what the remote viewer first described. MW The RV session is tape recorded and pen and paper are available for the remote viewer to sketch his perceptions. Experience has showni that some remote viewers prefer to combine written and oral Wi descriptions, while some prefer to work sequentially. The average RV session is approximately 30 minutes and adI never exceeds 60 minutes. (4) POST-SESSION ANALYSIS After the RV session is over, the remote viewer and interviewer obtain from the TPS specific information about the target and compare their session results with these data. The remote viewer and the interviewer discuss the session results. The purpose of this post- session analysis is to provide the remote viewer with the satisfaction of knowing how well he or she did. 8. (S) VARIATIONS IN PROCEDURE FOR RV a. General Remote Viewing The foregoing has described the use of coordinates to obtain from a remote viewer the description of that site. Another approach to the same goal is to access the target site on the basis of focusing on a person rather than a coordinate. For example, the remote viewer is provided some personal information and then proceeds to describe the location of the individual. Thus, the individual serves as a beacon to locate the target by RV. To standardize this approach, the procedure described in paragraph 6 is modified as follows. 4 SECRET Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 Approved For Release 2003gE1C DP96-00788RO01 300070003-9 QL (I (S) The elements of this procedure consist of (1) target selection; (2) remote viewer session preliminaries; (3) activity of person who serves as beacon; (4) remote viewing session; and (5) post-session analysis. (1) TARGET SELECTION A target pool of 50 to 100 targets will be selected by a TPS. The targets chosen will be distinctive, but to include more than one example of each type. This precludes the remote viewer from eliminating a target type because one example was used before. The remote viewer is informed that the target pool consists of similar as well as different types of targets. All other aspects of the target selection element of the procedure remain the same. (2) REMOTE VIEWER SESSION PRELIMINARIES This element is identical to that of the basic RV procedure. (3) ACTIVITY OF PERSON WHO SERVES AS BEACON At the beginning of the RV session, the remote viewer and interviewer are given one or more items of biographical information con- cerning, or may even meet briefly (for 3 to 5 minutes), the individual serving as the beacon. If the latter is the case, the beacon individual departs the meeting and obtains the target from the TPS. This procedure eliminates the possibility of the beacon individual divulging any hint of the target. The beacon individual travels to the target, arriving there at a previously specified time. He or she then interacts with the site for the predetermined length of time of the RV session. (4) REMOTE VIEWING SESSION This element is identical to that of the basic RV procedure. (5) POST-SESSION ANALYSIS This element is identical to that of the basic RV procedure. 5 SECRET Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 Approved For Release 2003§ft ft-fDP96-00788R001 300070003-9 (S) b. Tactical Remote Viewing Currently envisioned military RV applications will draw on general RV techniques utilized in coordinate and beacon RV. To stan- dardize this approach the procedures described in paragraph 6 and 7a is aw modified, as follows. (1) TARGET SELECTION Tactical targets will be selected from U.S. military units involved in military exercises; they will range over field test units with military vehicles such as tanks and helicopters which are instrumented to determine their spatial location throughout a field trial, operational units involved in routine field exercises and small units likely to undertake no-notice exercises. (2) REMOTE VIEWING SESSION PRELIMINARIES This element consists of the basic RV procedure, augmented by a briefing on some known aspects of the target. (3) ACTIVITY AT TARGET Target activity will be as prescribed by the commanders and umpires and will be totally independent of the RV session. (4) REMOTE VIEWING SESSION This element is identical to that of the basic RV procedure. (5) POST-SESSION ANALYSIS Mi After the RV session is over, the remote viewer and inter- viewer discuss the session results and formulate specific questions about the target activity, location, and state during the period of RV. When information on the military exercise is available the interviewer and the remote viewer compare it with the session results. The purpose of this post-session analysis is to provide the remote viewer with the satisfac- tion of knowing how well the remote viewer performed. 6 SECRET Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 Approved For Release 20039MWEIDP96-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 9. (S) SCOPE OF TARGETS FOR REMOTE VIEWING AMSAA-sponsored RV will exclude non program-associated U.S., allied, or neutral nation's citizens as targets. 10. (S) PERFORMING ORGANIZATION SRI International is the performing organization. 11. (S) PROJECTED MAXIMUM NUMBER OF REMOTE VIEWERS AND INTERVIEWERS & 8 active duty Army military officers 0 8 DA civilians (GS 12 and above) 0 3 AMSAA civilian consultants (retired general officers) 0 3 SRI International employees 0 9 SRI International Consultants Persons involved will principally occupy the role of either remote viewer or interviewer, but there may be some exchange of roles. 12. (S) SELECTION OF REMOTE VIEWERS AND INTERVIEWERS A number of Army personnel were introduced to the RV phenomena by a guest speaker presentation on the subject. Following this initial introductionp others became familiar with the phenomena through the circulation of open literature publications on RV. Discussions about the military applications of RV phenomena by interested analysts resulted in a decision by AMSAA management to seek establishment of a program to define the military utility of the process. Individuals who had previously shown an interest in the potential application of the RV process were invited to participate as a remote viewer or interviewer. Individuals desiring to participate in these tasks were accepted. Other individuals selected after the initial participants were identified were given an orientation on the phenomena and asked to read published materials on RV. After a familiarization with the RV process and procedures, individuals were asked if they would be willing to participate as a remote viewer or interviewer. Only those individuals who indicated a positive desire to participate were accepted. 7 to SECRET on Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 No Approved For Release 2003 U1 Ollff P96-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 9 E V. Wi In addition to Army personnel, SRI International has additional individuals who are participating as remote viewers; some of these are consultants, others are members of the SRI staff. As with the Army participants, only those individuals who indicated a positive desire to participate after familiarization with the RV process and procedures were accepted. An information sheet for participants is included here as Tab D. 13. (S) REPLICATION OF THE RV PROCESS It is proposed to conduct up to 40 RV sessions per month, up to a maximum of 200 RV sessions during the contract year. For each participant, the number of RV sessions is not to exceed 2 per day, and not to exceed 10 per week. 14. (S) EVALUATION The recorded RV images are independently evaluated by one or more judges in the following manner to semi-quantitatively determine the 00 correspondence of the RV results to the intended remote targets. 1. Determine the principle concepts (PC) stated in the images EW recorded in each RV session transcript. 2. At each target site blind judge the correspondence, on a No scale of 0 to 10, of each PC of each transcript. 3. Calculate the mean and variance of all the judged PCs of mi each RV session for each target. 4. Rank the correspondence of each RV session for each target using the calculated mean values. 5. Review the correspondence ranking utilizing any drawings Wd or sketches to fine-tune the rankings to establish a final judged rank ordering of each RV session with each target. 15. (S) CONFIDENTIALITY Individuals performing as remote viewers and interviewers under the SRI International GRILL FLAME programs will not be identified without their prior consent and they will be referred to in project records only by an WW alpha-numeric designator. Products of remote viewers and interviewer such SECRET Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 Approved For Release 200,ztn DP96-00788RO01 300070003-9 OW SR. ifff (S) as tapes, drawings, transcripts , rosters, or other materials which might reveal the identity of the remote viewer will be coded to assure the protection of their identity. 16. (S) PHYSICAL ENVIRONMENT RV sessions will be conducted in an ordinary room at ambient tempera- ture and humidity during the normal waking hours of the participants. 9 SECRET Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 SECRET ou am (S) BIBLIOGRAPHY 1. Puthoff, H. E. and Targ, R., "A Perceptual Channel for Information Transfer Over Kilometer Distances: Historical Perspective and Recent Research," IEEE Proceedings, Vol. 64, No. 3 (March 1976). 11 2. Puthoff, H. E. and Targ, R., State of the Art in Remote Viewing Studies at SRI, 1977 Proceedings, IEEE, of International Conference on Cybernetics and Society. 3. Bisaha, J. P. and Dunne, B. J., "Multiple Subject and Long Distance Precognitive Remote Viewing of Geographical Locations," 1977 Proceedings, IEEE, of International Conferences on Cybernetics of Society. 4. Puthoff, H. E., Targ, R., and May, E. C., "(S) Advanced Threat W Technique Assessment (U)," Stanford Research Institute Final Report Covering Period from 14 April 1976 to 15 April 1977 (July 1977). 5. Puthoff, H. E., Targ, P., May, E. C. and Swann, I., "(S) Advanced Threat Technique Assessment (U)," SRI International Final Report, Covering Period from 18 April 1977 to 18 April 1978 (October 1978). did 10 SECRET Now Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 Approved For ReleaU ffft)k "ff rrff788RO01 300070003-9 i J TAB A PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, VOL. 64, NO. 3, MARCH 1976 (U) UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED 1111111 '%@J@&J%31/09/10: CIA-RDP96-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 PROCEEDINGS OF @WRY!VOFLO F E4 976 1101@: A Perceptual Channel for Information Transfer over Kilometer Distances: Historical Perspective CPYRGHT and Recent Research HAROLD E. PUTHOFF, MEMBER, IEEE, AND RUSSELL TARG, SENIOR MEMBER, IEEE 329 Abstract-For more than 100 years, scientists have attempted to determine the truth or falsity of claims for the existence of a percept!!al channel whereby certain individuals are able to perceive and describe remote data not presented to any known sense. This paper presents an outline of the history of scientific inquiry into such so-called paranor. mal perception and surveys the current state of the art in parapsycho- logical research in the United States and abroad. The nature of this perceptual channel is examined in a series of experiments carried out in the Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory of Stanford Research Institute. The perceptual modality most extensively investigated is the ability of both experienced subjects and inexperienced volunteers to view, by innate mental processes, remote geographical or technical targets including buildings, roads, and laboratory apparatus. The ac- cumulated data indicate that the phenomenon is not a sensitive func- tion of distance, and Faraday cage shielding does not in any apparent way degrade the quality and accuracy of perception. On the basis of this research, some areas of physics are suggested from which a descrip. tion or explanation of the phenomenon could be forthcoming. I. INTRODUCTION "IT IS THE PROVINCE of natural science to investigate nature, impartially and without prejudice" [I ] . Nowhere in scientific inquiry has this dictum met as great a chal- lenge as in the area of so-called extrasensory perception (ESP), the detection of remote stimuli not mediated by the usual sensory processes. Such phenomena, although under scientific consideration for over a century, have historically been fraught with unreliability and controversy, and validation of the phe- nomena by accepted scientific methodology has been slow in coming. Even so, a recent survey conducted by the British publication New Scientist revealed that 67 percent of nearly 1500 responding readers (the majority of whom are working scientists and technologists) considered ESP to be an estab- lished fact or a likely possibility, and 88 percent held the investigation of ESP to be a legitimate scientific undertaking [2]. A review of the literature reveals that although experiments by reputable researchers yielding positive results were begun over a century ago (e.g., Sir William Crookes' study of D. D. Home, 1860's) [31, many consider the study of these phe- nomena as only recently emerging from the realm of quasi- science. One reason for this is that, despite experimental results, no satisfactory theoretical construct had been advanced to correlate data or to predict new experimental outcomes. Consequently, the area in question remained for a long time in the recipe stage reminiscent of electrodynamics before the Manuscript received July 25, 1975; revised November 7, 105. The submission of this paper was encouraged after review of an advance proposal. This work was supported by the Foundation for Parasensory Investigation and the Parapsychology Foundation, New York, NY; the Institute of Noetic Sciences, Palo Alto, CA; and the National Aero- nautics and Space Administration, under Contract NAS 7-100. The authors are with the Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory, Stanford Research institute, Menlo Park, CA 94025. 111i unification brought about by the work of Ampere, Faraday, and Maxwell. Since the early work, however, we have seen the development of information theory, quantum theory, and neuro physiological research, and these disciplines provide powerful conceptual tools that appear to bear directly on the issue. In fact, several physicists (Section V) are now of the opinion that these phenomena are not at all inconsistent with the framework of modern physics: the often-held view that observations of this type are a priori incompatible with known laws is erroneous in that such a concept is based on the naive I realism prevalent before the development of quantum theory. In the emerging view, it is accepted that research in this area can be conducted so as to uncover not just a catalog of inter- esting events, but rather patterns of cause-effect relationships of the type that lend themselves to analysis and hypothesis in the forms with which we are familiar in the physical sciences. One hypothesis is that information transfer under conditions of sensory shielding is mediated by extremely low-frequency (ELF) electromagnetic waves, a proposal that does not seem to be ruled out by any obvious physical or biological facts. Further, the development of information theory makes it possible to characterize and quantify the performance of a communications channel regardless of the underlying mechanism. For the past three.years, we have had a program in the Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory of the Stan- ford Research Institute (SRI) to investigate those facets of human perception that appear to fall outside the range of well- understood perceptual/processing capabilities. Of particular interest is a human information-accessing capability that we call "remote viewing." This phenomenon pertains to the ability of certain individuals to access and describe, by means of mental processes, information sources blocked from ordi- nary perception, and generally accepted as secure against such access. In particular, the phenomenon we have investigated most extensively is the ability of a subject to view remote geograph- ical locations up to several thousand kilometers distant from his physical location (given only a known person on whom.to target).' We have carried out more than fifty experiments under controlled laboratory conditions with several individuals whose remote perceptual abilities have been developed suf- ficiently to allow them at times to describe correctly-often in great detail-geographical or technical material such as build- ings, roads, laboratory apparatus, and the like. As observed in the laboratory, the basic phenomenon appears to cover a range of subjective experiences variously referred to 'Our initial work in this area was reported in Nature [41, and re- printed in the IEEE Commun. Soc. Newsletter, vol. 13, Jan. 1975. UNCLASSIFIED CPYRGHT 330 UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 or Fig. 1. Airport in San Andres, Colombia, used as remote-viewing target, along with sketch produced by subject in California. in the literature as autoscopy (in the medical literature); exteri- orization or disassociation (psychological literature); simple clairvoyance, traveling clairvoyance, or out-of-body experience (parapsychological literature); or astral projection (occult liter- ature). We choose the term "remote viewing" as a neutral descriptive term free from prior associations and bias as to mechanisms. The development at SRI of a successful experimental pro- cedure to elicit this capability has evolved to the point where persons such as visiting government scientists and contract monitors, with no previous exposure to such concepts, have learned to perform well; and subjects who have trained over a one-year period have performed excellently under a variety of experimental conditions. Our accumulated data thus indicate that both specially selected and unselected persons can be assisted in developing remote perceptual abilities up to a level of useful information transfer. In experiments of this type, we have three principal findings. First, we have established that it is possible to obtain signifi- cant amounts of accurate descriptive information about remote locations. Second, an increase in the distance from a few meters up to 4000 km separating the subject from the scene to be perceived does not in any apparent way degrade the quality or accuracy of perception. Finallyi the use of Faraday cage electrical shielding does not prevent high-quality descrip- tions from being obtained. To build a coherent theory for the explanation of these phenomena, it is necessary to have a clear understanding of what constitutes the phenomena. In this paper, we first briefly summarize previous efforts in this field in Section 11. We then present in Sections III and IV the results of a series of more than fifty experiments with nine subjects carried out in our own laboratory, which represent a sufficiently stable data base to permit testing of various hypotheses concerning the func- tioning of this channel. Finally, in Section V, we indicate those areas of physics and information theory that appear to be relevant to an understanding of certain aspects of the phenomena. First, however, we present an illustrative example generated in an early pilot experiment. As will be clear from our later discussion, this is not a "best-ever" example, but rather a typical sample of the level of proficiency that can be reached and that we have come to expect in our research. Three subjects participated in a long-distance experiment focusing on a series of targets in Costa Rica. These subjects said they had never been to Costa Rica. In this experiment, one of the experimenters (Dr. Puthoff ) spent ten days traveling through Costa Rica on a combination business/pleasure trip. This information was all that was known to the subjects about the traveler's itinerary. The experiment called for Dr. Puthoff to keep a detailed record of his location and activities, includ- ing photographs of each of seven target days at 1330 PDT. A total of twelve daily descriptions were collected before the traveler's return: six responses from one subject, five from another, and one from a third. The third subject who submitted the single response supplied a drawing for a day in the middle of the series. (The subject's response, together with the photographs taken at the site, are shown in Fig. 1). Although Costa Rica is a mountainous country, the subject unexpectedly perceived the traveler at a beach and ocean setting. With some misgiving, he described an airport on a sandy beach and an airstrip with the ocean at the Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED 41 1 aA MR 4 4, oc", /:.2,; CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 PUTHOFF AND TARG: PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER 331 end (correct). An airport building also was drawn, and shown to have a large rectangular overhang (correct). The traveler had taken an unplanned one-day side trip to an offshore island and at the time of the experiment had just disembarked from a plane at a small island airport as described by the subject 4000 km away. The sole discrepancy was that the subject's drawing showed a Quonset-hut type of building in place of the rectangular structure. The above description was chosen as an example to illustrate a major point observed a number of times throughout the program to be described. Contrary to what may be expected, a subject's description does not necessarily portray what may reasonably be expected to be correct (an educated or "safe" guess), but often runs counter even to the subject's own expectations. We wish to stress again that a result such as the above is not unusual. The remaining submissions in this experiment pro- vided further examples of excellent correspondences between target and response. (A target period of poolside relaxation was identified; a drive through a tropical forest at the base of a truncated volcano was described as a drive through a jungle below a large bare table mountain; a hotel-room target descrip- tion, including such details as rug color, was correct; and so on.) So as to determine whether such matches were simply fortuitous-that is, could reasonably be expected on the basis of chance alone-Dr. Puthoff was asked after he had returned to blind match the twelve descriptions to his seven target locations. On the basis of this conservative evaluation proce- dure, which vastly underestimates the statistical significance of the individual descriptions, five correct matches were ob- tained. :his number of matches is significant at p = 0.02 by exact binomial calculation. 2 The observation of such unexpectedly high-quality descrip- tions early in our program led to a large-scale study of the phenomenon at SRI under secure double-blind conditions (i.e., target unknown to experimenters as well as subjects), with independent random target selection and blind judging. The results, presented in Sections III and IV, provide strong evi- dence for the robustness of this phenomenon whereby a human perceptual modality of extreme sensitivity can detect complex remote stimuli. 11. BACKGROUND Although we are approaching the study of these phenomena as physicists, it is not yet possible to separate ourselves entirely from the language of the nineteenth century when'the labora- tory study of the paranormal was begun. Consequently, we continue to use terms such as "paranormal," "telepathy," and the like. However, we intend only to indicate a process of information transfer under conditions generally accepted as secure against such transfer and with no prejudice or occult assumptions as to the mechanisms involved. As in any other scientific pursuit, the purpose is to collect the observables that result from experiments and to try to determine the functional relationships between these observables and the laws of physics as they are currently understood. 2The probability of a correct daily match by chance for any given transcript is p = -L. Therefore, the probability of at least five correct 7 matches by chance out of twelve tries can be calculated from 12 121 /l\i16@(12-i) P = Z Tl-(- - = 0.02. i=5 12 @7) Organized research into so-called psychic functioning began roughly in the time of J. J. Thomson, Sir Oliver Lodge, and Sir William Crookes, all of whom took part in the founding of the Society for Psychical Research (SPR) in 1882 in England. Crookes, for example, carried out his principal investigations with D. D. Home, a Scotsman who grew up in America and returned to England in 1855 [3]. According to the notebooks and published reports of Crookes, Home had demonstrated the ability to cause objects to move without touching them. We should note in passing that, Home, unlike most subjects, worked only in the light and spoke out in the strongest pos- sible terms against the darkened seance rooms popular at the time [5]. Sir William Crookes was a pioneer in the study of electrical discharge in gases and in the development of vacuum tubes, some types of which still bear his name. Although everything Crookes said about electron beams and plasmas was accepted, nothing he said about the achievements of D. D. Home ever achieved that status. Many of his colleagues, who had not observed the experiments with Home, stated publicly that they thought Crookes had been deceived, to which Crookes angrily responded: Will not my critics give me credit for some amount of common sense9 Do they not imagine that the obvious precautions, which occur' to them as soon as they sit down to pick holes in my experiments, have occurred to me also in the course of mypro- longed and patient investigation? The answer to this, as to all other objections is, prove it to be an error, by showing where the error lies, or if a trick, by showing how the trick is per- formed. Try the experiment fully and fairly. If then fraud be found, expose it; if it be a truth, proclaim it. This is the only scientific procedure, and it is that I propose steadily to pursue 131. In the United States, scientific interest in the paranormal was centered in the universities. In 1912, John Coover [61 was established in the endowed Chair of Psychical Research at Stanford University. In the 1920's, Harvard University set up research programs with George Estabrooks and L. T. Troland (71, [8]. It was'in this framework that, in 1930, William McDougall invited Dr. J. B. Rhine and Dr. Louisa Rhine to join the Psychology Department at Duke University [9]. For more than 30 years, significant work was carried out at Rhine's Duke University Laboratory. To examine the existence of paranormal perception, he used the now-famous ESP cards containing a boldly printed picture of a star, cross, square, circle, or wavy lines. Subjects were asked to name the order of these cards in a freshly shuffled deck of twenty-five such cards. To test for telepathy, an experimenter would look at the cards one at a time, and a subject suitably separated from the sender would attempt to determine which card was being viewed. Dr. J. B. Rhine together with Dr. J. G. Pratt carried out thousands of experiments of this type under widely varying conditions[101. The statistical results from these experiments indicated that some individuals did indeed possess a paranor- mal perceptual ability in that it was possible to obtain an arbitrarily high degree of improbability by continued testing of a gifted subject. The work of Rhine has been challenged on many grounds, however, including accusations of improper handling of statis- tics, error, and fraud. With regard to the statistics, the general consensus of statisticians today is that if fault is to be found in Rhine's work, it would have to be on other than statistical grounds [I 11. With regard to the accusations of fraud, the UNCLASSIFIED CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 332 reinforcement, he developed several outstanding subjects, one of whom, Pavel Stepanek, has worked with experimenters around the world for more than 10 years. Ryzl's pioneering work came as an answer to the questions raised by the 1956 CIBA Foundation conference on extra- sensory perception. The CIBA Chemical Company has annual meetings on topics of biological and chemical interest, and that same year they assembled several prominent parapsy- chologists to- have a state-of-the-art conference on ESP [23). The conference concluded that little progress would be made in parapsychology research until a repeatable experiment could be found; namely, an experiment that different experi- menters could repeat at will and that would reliably yield a statistically significant result. RyzI had by 1962 accomplished that goal. His primary con- tribution was a decision to interact with the subject as a per- son, to try to build up his confidence and ability. His protocol depended on "working with" rather than "running" his sub- jects. Ryzl's star subject, Pavel Stepanek, has produced highly significant results with many contemporary researchers [ 241 - [29]. In these experiments, he was able to tell with 60-percent reliability whether a hidden card was green side or white side up, yielding statistics of a million to one with only a thousand trials. As significant as such results are statistically, the information channel is imperfect, containing noise along with the signal. When considering how best to use such a channel, one is led to the communication theory concept of the introduction of redundancy as a means of coding a message to combat the effects of a noisy channel [301. A prototype experiment by RyzI using such techniques has proved to be successful. RyzI had an assistant select randomly five groups of three digits each. These 15 digits were then encoded into binary form and translated into a sequence of green and white cards in sealed envelopes. By means of repeated calling and an elaborate majority vote protocol, RyzI was able after 19 350 calls by Stepanek (averaging 9 s per call) to correctly identify all 15 numbers, a result significant at p = 10-15 The hit rate for individual calls was 61.9 percent, 11 978 hits, and 7372 misses [311. Note Added in Proof- It has been brought to our attention that a similar procedure was recently used to transmit without error the word "peace" in International Morse Code (J. C. Carpenter, "Toward the effective utilization of enhanced weak-signal ESP effects," presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, New York, NY, Jan. 27, 1975). The characteristics of such a channel can be specified in accordance with the precepts of communication theory. The bit rate associated with the information channel is calculated from [301 most celebrated case of criticism of Rhine's work, that of G. R. Price [ 12 1, ended 17 years after it began when the accusation of fraud was retracted by its author in an article entitled "Apology to Rhine and Soal," published in the same journal in which it was first put forward [ 13 ]. It should also be noted that parapsychological researchers themselves re- cently exposed fraud in their own laboratory when they encountered it [ 14 At the end of the 1940's, Prof. S. G. Soal, an English mathe- matician working with the SPR, had carried out hundreds of card guessing experiments involving tens of thousands of calls [151. Many of these experiments were carried out over ex- tended distances. One of the most notable experiments was conducted with Mrs. Gloria Stewart between London and Antwerp. This experiment gave results whose probability of occurring by chance were less than 10-8. With the publication of Modern Experiments in Telepathy by Soal and Bateman (both of whom were statisticians), it appeared that card guess- ing experiments produced significant results, on the average. 3 The most severe criticism of all this work, a criticism diffi- cult to defend against in principle, is that leveled by the well- known British parapsychological critic C. E. M. Hansel [ 17], who began his examination of the ESP hypothesis with the stated assumption, "In view of the a priori arguments against it we know in advance that telepathy, etc., cannot occur." Therefore, based on the "a priori unlikelihood" of ESP, Hansel's examination of the literature centered primarily on the possibility of fraud, by subjects or, investigators. He reviewed in depth four experiments which he regarded as providing the best evidence of ESP: the Pearce-Pratt distance series [181; the Pratt-Woodruff [19] series, both conducted at Duke; and Soal's work with Mrs. Stewart and Basil Shackle- ton [IS I, as well as a more recent series by Soal and Bowden [20]. Hansel showed, in each case, how fraud could have been committed (by the experimenters in the Pratt-Woodruff and Soal-Bateman series, or by the subjects in the Pearce-Pratt and Soal-Bowden experiments). He gave no direct evidence that fraud was committed in these experiments, but said, "If the result could have arisen through a trick, the experiment must be considered unsatisfactory proof of ESP, whether or not it is finally decided that such a trick was in fact used " f 17, p. 18). As discussed by Honorton in a review of the field [21 ], Hansel's conclusion after 241 pages of careful scrutiny therefore was that these experiments were not "fraud-proof" and therefore in principle could not serve as conclusive proof of ESP. Even among the supporters of ESP research and its results, there remained the consistent problem that many successful subjects eventually lost their ability and their scores gradually drifted toward chance results. This decline effect in no way erased their previous astronomical success; but it was a disap- pointment since if paranormal perception is a natural ability, one would like to see subjects improving with practice rather than getting worse. One of the first successful attempts to overcome the decline effect was in Czechoslovakia in the work of Dr. Milan Ryzl, a chemist with the Institute of Biology of the Czechoslovakian Academy of Science and also an amateur hypnotist [221. Through the use of hypnosis, together with feedback and 3Recently, some of the early Soal experiments have been criticized -distance experiments cited here were judged 116]. However, his long in a double-blind fashion of the type that escaped the criticism of the early experiments. PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 R = H(x) - Hy(x) where H(x) is the uncertainty of the source message containing symbols with a priori probability pi: 2 H(x), Pi 1092 Pi (2) and Hy W is the conditional entropy based on the a posteriori probabilities that a received signal was actually transmitted: 2 H E P 0, j) 1092 Pi W - (3) Y(X) U-1 UNCLASSIF-I-ED, CPYRGHT UNCLASSIF IED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 PUTHOFF AND TARG: PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER no am 333 1 For Stepanek's run, with pi = _T , pi (j) = 0.619, and an average time of 9 s per choice, we have a source uncertainty H(x) = I bit and a calculated bit rate or R - 0.041 bit/symbol RIT - 0.0046 bit/s. (Since the 15-digit number (49.8 bits) actually was transmitted at the rate of 2.9 X 10-4 bit/s, an increase in bit rate by a factor of about 20 could be expected on the basis of a coding scheme more optimum than that used in the experiments. See, for example, Appendix A.) Dr. Charles Tart at the University of California has written extensively on the so-called decline effect. He considers that having subjects attempt to guess cards, or perform any other repetitious task for which they receive no feedback, follows the classical technique for deconditioning any response. He thus considers card guessing "a technique for extinguishing psychic functioning in the laboratory" [ 32 1. Tart's injunctions of the mid-sixties were being heeded at Maimonides Hospital, Brooklyn, NY, by a team of researchers that included Dr. Montague Ullman, who was director of research for the hospital; Dr. Stanley Krippner; and, later, Charles Honorton. These three worked together for several years on experiments on the occurrence of telepathy in dreams. In. the course of a half-dozen experimental series, they found in their week-long sessions a number of subjects who had dreams that consistently were highly descriptive of pictorial material that a remote sender was looking at throughout the night. This work is described in detail in the experimenters' book Dream Telepathy [331, Honorton is continuing work of this free-response type in which the subject has no precon- ceived idea as to what the target may be. In his more recent. work with subjects in the waking state, Honorton is providing homogeneous stimulation to the subject who is to describe color slides viewed by another person in a remote room. In this new work, the subject listens to white noise via earphones and views an homogeneous visual field imposed through the use of Ping-Pong ball halves to cover the subject's eyes in conjunction with diffuse ambient illumina- tion. In this so-called Ganzfeld setting, subjects are again able, now in the waking state, to give correct and often highly accurate descriptions of the material being viewed by the sender [34]. In Honorton's work and elsewhere, it apparently has been the step away from the repetitive forced-choice experiment that has opened the way for a wide variety of ordinary people to demonstrate significant functioning in the laboratory, with- out being bored into a decline effect. This survey would be incomplete if we did not indicate certain aspects of the current state of research in the USSR. It is clear from translated documents and other sources [35] that many laboratories in the USSR arc engaged in paranormal research. Since the 1930's, in the laboratory of L. Vasiliev (Leningrad Institute for Brain Research), there has been an interest in the use of telepathy as a method of influencing the behavior of a person at a distance. In Vasiliev's book Experiments in Mentdl Suggestion, he makes it very clear that the bulk of his labora- tory's experiments were aimed at long-distance communica- tion combined with a form of behavior modification; for example, putting people at a distance to sleep through hyp- nosis [36]. Similar behavior modification types of experiments have been carried out in recent times by I. M. Kogan, Chairman of the Bioinformation Section of the Moscow Board of the Popov Society. He is a Soviet engineer who, until 1969, published extensively on the theory of telepathic communication [371- [40 1. He was concerned with three principal kinds of experi- ments: mental suggestion without hypnosis over short dis- tances, in which the percipient attempts to identify an object; mental awakening over short distances, in which a subject is awakened from a hypnotic sleep at the "beamed" suggestion from the hypnotist; and long-range (intercity) telepathic com- munication. Kogan's main interest has been to quantify the channel capacity of the paranormal channel. He finds that the bit rate decreases from 0.1 bit/s for laboratory experiments to 0.005 bit/s for his 1000-krn intercity experiments. In the USSR, serious consideration is given to the hypothesis that telepathy is mediated by extremely low-frequency (ELF) electromagnetic propagation. (The pros and cons of this hypothesis are discussed in Section V of this paper.) In general, the entire field of paranormal research in the USSR is part of a larger one concerned with the interaction between electromagnetic fields and living organisms [411, [42]. At the First International Congress on Parapsychology and Psychotronics in Prague, Czechoslovakia, in 1973, for example, Kholodov spoke at length about the susceptibility of living systems to extremely low-level ac and dc fields. He described conditioning effects on the behavior of fish resulting from the application of 10 to 100 pW of RF to their tank [43]. The USSR take these data seriously in that the Soviet safety re- quirements for steady-state microwave exposure set limits at 10 tLW/cm 2, whereas the United States has set a steady-state limit of 10 mW/cm 2 [441. Kholodov spoke also about the nonthermal effects of microwaves on animals' central nervous systems. His experiments were very carefully carried out and are characteristic of a new dimension in paTanormal research. The increasing importance of this area in Soviet research was indicated recently when the Soviet Psychological Association issued an unprecedented position paper calling on the Soviet Academy of Sciences to step up efforts in this area [45 ]. They recommended that the newly formed Psychological Institute within the Soviet Academy of Sciences and the Psychological Institute of the Academy of Pedagogical Sciences review the area and consider the creation of a new laboratory within one of the institutes to study persons with unusual abilities. They also recommended a comprehensive evaluation of experiments and theory by the Academy of Sciences' Insti- tute of Biophysics and Institute for the Problems of Informa- tion Transmission. The Soviet research, along with other behavioristically oriented work, suggests that in addition to obtaining overt responses such as verbalizations or key presses from a subject, it should be possible to obtain objective evidence of informa- tion transfer by direct measurement of physiological parame- ters of a subject. Kamiya, Lindsley, Pribram, Silverman, Walter, and others brought together to discuss physiological methods to detect ESP functioning, have suggested that a whole range of electroencephalogram (EEG) responses such as evoked potentials (EP's), spontaneous EEG, and the contingent negative variation (CNV) might be sensitive indicators of the detection of- remote stimuli not mediated by usual sensory processes [46]. Early experimentation of this type was carried out by Douglas Dean at the Newark College of Engineering. In his UNCLASSIFIED CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 334 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 search for physiological correlates of information transfer, he 0 used the plethysmograph to measure changes in the blood C 0 volume in a finger a sensitive indicator of autonomic nervous , system functioning [471. A plethysmographic measurement J was made on the finger of a subject during telepathy experi- -6 - 6 ments. A sender looked at randomly selected target cards ca 16 16 L consisting of names known to the subject, together with names I unknown to him (selected at random from a telephone book). I The names of the known people were contributed by the sub- LU (D ject and were to be of emotional significance to him. Dean < found significant changes in the chart recording of finger _J 0 blood volume when the remote sender was looking at those > names known to the subject as compared with those names randomly chosen. 5 Hz 10 Hz 15 Hz Three other experiments using the physiological approach Fig. 2. Occipital EEG frequency spectra, 0-20 Hz, of one subject (H.H.) have now been published. The first work by Tart [48 1, a later acting as receiver showing amplitude changes in the 9-1 1.Hz band as a function of strobe frequency. Three cases: 0-, 6-, and 16-Hz flashes work by Lloyd [491, and most recently the work by the (twelve trial averages). authors [41 all follow a similar procedure. Basically, a subject is closeted in an electrically shielded room while his EEG is results were produced by system artifacts, electromagnetic recorded. Meanwhile, in another laboratory, a second person pickup (EMI), or subtle cueing; the results were negative [4]. is stimulated from time to time, and the time of that stimulus As part of the experimental protocol, the subject was asked is marked on the magnetic-tape recording of the subject's EEG. to indicate a conscious assessment for each trial (via telegraph The subject does not know when the remote stimulus periods key) as to the nature of the stimulus; analysis showed these are as compared with the nonstimulus periods. guesses to be at chance. Thus arousal as evidenced by signifi- With regard to choice of stimulus for our own experimenta- cant alpha blocking occurred only at the noncognitive level of tion, we noted that in previous work others had attempted, physiological response. Hence the experiment provided direct without success, to detect evoked potential changes in a sub- physiological (EEG) evidence of perception of remote stimuli ject's EEG in response to a single stroboscopic flash stimulus even in the absence of overt cognitive response. observed by another subject [50]. In a discussion of that Whereas in our experiments we used a remote light flash as a experiment, Kamiya suggested that because of the unknown stimulus, Tart [481 in his work used an electrical shock to temporal characteristics of the information channel, it might himself as sender, and Lloyd [491 simply told the sender to be more appropriate to use repetitive bursts of light to increase think of a red triangle each time a red warning light was the probability of detecting information transfer [ 5 11. There- illuminated within his view. Lloyd observed a consistent fore, in our study we chose to use a stroboscopic flash train of evoked potential in his subjects; whereas in our experiments I 0-s duration as the remote stimulus. and in Tart's, a reduction in amplitude and a desynchroniza- In the design of the study, we assumed that the application tion of alpha was observed-an arousal response. (if a subject of the remote stimulus would result in responses similar to is resting in an alpha-dominant condition and he is then those obtained under conditions of direct stimulation. For stimulated, for example in any direct manner, one will observe example, when an individual is stimulated with a low- a desynchronization and decrease in alpha power.) We con- frequency (< 30 Hz) flashing light, the EEG typically shows sider that these combined results are evidence for the existence a decrease in the amplitude of the resting rhythm and a of noncognitive awareness of remote happenings and that they driving of the brain waves at the frequency of the flashes [52 1. have a profound implication for paranormal research. We hypothesized that if we stimulated one subject in this SRI INVESTIGATIONS OF REMOTE VIEWING III manner (a putative sender), the EEG of another subject in a . remote room with no flash present (a receiver) might show Experimentation in remote viewing began during studies changes in alpha (9-11 Hz) activity and possibly an EEG carried out to investigate the abilities of a New York artist, driving similar to that of the sender, or other coupling to the Ingo Swann, when he expressed the opinion that the insights sender's EEG [531. The receiver was seated in a visually gained during experiments at SRI had strengthened his ability opaque, acoustically and electrically shielded, double-walled (verified in other research before he joined the SRI program) steel room about 7 rn from the sender. The details of the to view remote locations [54]. To test Mr. Swann's asser- experiment, consisting of seven runs of thirty-six 10-s trials tion, a pilot study was set up in which a series of targets from each (twelve periods each for 0-Hz, 6-Hz, and 16-Hz stimuli, around the globe were supplied by SRI personnel to the ex- randomly intermixed), are presented in [4]. This experiment perimenters on a double-blind. basis. Mr. Swann's apparent proved to be successful. The receiver's alpha activity (9-11 Hz) ability to describe correctly details of buildings, roads, showed a significant reduction in average power (-24 percent, bridges, and the like indicated that it may be possible for p < 0.04) and peak power (-28 percent, p < 0.03) during a subject by means of mental imagery to access and describe 16-Hz flash stimuli as compared with periods of no-flash randomly chosen geographical sites located several miles stimulus. [A similar response was observed for 6-11z stimuli from the subject's position and demarcated by some appro- 12 percent in average power, - 21 percent in peak power), priate means. Therefore, we set up a research program to but the latter result did not reach statistical significance.] test the remote-viewing hypothesis under rigidly controlled Fig. 2 shows an overlay of three averaged EEG spectra from scientific conditions. one of the subject's 36 trial runs, displaying differences in In carrying out this program, we concentrated on what we alpha activity during the three stimulus conditions. Extensive considered to be our principal responsibility-to resolve under control procedures Xere undeTtAken to determin 'f th proved Foi 20bi 09ffb: p conditio bdd*8 Aft2f cyhether or not this 'WbV4 0 0 7 8 8 WOM 3 8YA- 9 1" CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Ini PUTHOFF AND TAR8PWRC%4LFACLr&R%aeF;RONPX'MRTI &V@-PMA-00788RO01 300070003-9 335 IN class of paranormal perception phenomenon exists. At all times, we and others responsible for the overall program took measures to prevent sensory leakage and subliminal cueing and to prevent deception, whether intentional or unintentional. To ensure evaluations independent of belief structures of both experimenters and judges, all experiments were carried out under a protocol, described below, in which target selection at the beginning of experiments and blind judging of results at the end of experiments were handled independently of the researchers engaged in carrying out the experiments. Six subjects, designated S I through S6, were chosen for the study. Three were considered as gifted or experienced subjects (S 1 through S 3), and three were considered as learners (S 4 through S6). The a priori dichotomy between gifted and learners was based on the experienced group having been successful in other studies conducted before this program and the learners group being inexperienced with regard to paranormal. experimentation. The study consisted of a series of double-blind tests with local targets in the San Francisco Bay Area so that several in- dependent judges could visit the sites to establish documenta- tion. The protocol was to closet the subject with an experi- menter at SRI and at an agreed-on time to obtain from the subject a description of an undisclosed remote site being visited by a target team. In each of the experiments, one of the six program subjects served as remote-viewing subject, and SRI experimenters served as a target demarcation team at the remote location chosen in a double-blind protocol as follows. In each experiment, SRI management randomly chose a target location from a list of targets within a 30-min driving time from SRI; the target location selected was kept blind to subject and experimenters. The target pool consisted of more than 100 target locations chosen from a target-rich environ- ment. (Before the experimental series began, the Director of the Information Science and Engineering Division, not other- wise associated with the experiment, established the set of lo- cations as the target pool which remained known only to him * The target locations were printed on cards sealed in envelopes and kept in the SRI Division office safe. They were available only with the personal assistance of the Division Director who issued a single random-number selected target card that con- stituted the traveling orders for that experiment.) In detail: To begin the experiment, the subject was closeted with an experimenter at SRI to wait 30 min before beginning a narrative description of the remote location. A second ex- perimenter then obtained from the Division Director a target location from a set of traveling orders previously prepared and randomized by the Director and kept under his control. The target demarcation team, consisting of two to four SRI experi- menters, then proceeded by automobile directly to the target without any communication with the subject or experimenter remaining behind. The experimenter remaining with the sub- ject at SRI was kept ignorant of both the particular target and the target pool so as to eliminate the possibility of cueing (overt or subliminal) and to allow him freedom in questioning the subject to clarify his descriptions. The demarcation team remained at the target site for an agreed-on I 5-min period following the 30 min allotted for travel.4 During the observa- 4 The first subject (SI) was allowed 30 min for his descriptions, but it was found that he fatigued and had little comment after the first 15 min. The viewing time was therefore reduced to 15 min for subjects S2 through S6. tion period, the remote-viewing subject was asked to describe his impressions of the target site into a tape recorder and to make any drawings he thought appropriate. An informal com- parison was then made when the demarcation team returned, and the subject was taken to the site to provide feedback. A. Subject Sl: Experienced To begin the series, Pat Price, a former California police com- missioner and city councilman, participated as a subject in nine experiments. In general, Price's ability to describe correctly buildings, docks, roads, gardens, and the like, includ- ing structural materials, color, ambience, and activity-often in great detail-indicated the functioning of a remote per- ceptual ability. A Hoover Tower target, for example, was recognized and named by name. Nonetheless, in general, the descriptions contained inaccuracies as well as correct state- ments. A typical example is indicated by the subject's drawing shown in Fig. 3 in which he correctly described a park-like area containing two pools of water: one rectangular, 60 by 89 ft (actual dimensions 75 by 100 ft); the other circular, diameter 120 ft (actual diameter 110 ft). He incorrectly indi- cated the function, however, as water filtration rather than recreational swimming. (We often observe essentially correct descriptions of basic elements and patterns coupled with in- complete or erroneous analysis of function.) As can be seen from his drawing, he also included some elements, such as the tanks shown in the upper right, that are not present at the target site. We also note an apparent left-right reversal, often observed in paranormal perception experiments. To obtain a numerical evaluation of the accuracy of the remote-viewing experiment, the experimental results were subjected to independent judging on a blind basis by an SRI research analyst not otherwise associated with the research. The subject's response packets, which contained the nine typed unedited transcripts of the tape-recorded narratives along with any associated drawings, were unlabeled and pre- sented in random order. While standing at each target loca- tion, visited in turn; the judge was required to blind rank order the nine packets on a scale I to 9 (best to worst match). The statistic of interest is the sum of ranks assigned to the target- associated transcripts, lower values indicating better matches. For nine targets, the sum of ranks could range from nine to eighty-one. The probability that a given sum of ranks s or less will occur by chance is given by [ 5 5 k Pr (s or less) = _n E Y_ (- 1) 1 n) (I - NI - 1) N i=n 1=0 (I n- I where s is obtained sum of ranks, N is number of assignable ranks, n is number of occasions on whichims were made, and I takes on values from zero to the L il, Velinteger k in (i - n)IN. (Table I is a table to enable easy application of the above formula to those cases in which N = n.) The sum in this case, which included seven direct hits out of the nine, was 16 (see Table 11), a result significant at p =.2.9 X 10-5 by exact calculation. In Experiments 3, 4, and 6 through 9, the subject was se- cured in a double-walled copper-screen Faraday cage. The Faraday cage provides 120-dB attenuation for plane-wave radio-frequency radiation over a range of 15 kHz to I GHz. For magnetic fields, the attenuation is 68 dB at 15 kHz and decreases to 3 dB at 60 Hz. The results of rank order judging (Table II) indicate that the use of Faraday cage electrical UNCLASSIFIED CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 336 PROCEEDINGS OF THE _J_J AV E. ELECT TENNIS COURTS DIST. 0767(D L 1W PLAY GROUNDS POOL HOUSE SERVICE CONCRETE BLOCK YARD FIRE STA'nON PICNI AREA/ - 10D - Fig. 3 (a) Swimming pool complex as remote-viewing target (b) (a) City map of target location. (b) Drawing by Price (Sl). TABLE I CRITICAL VALUES OF Sums OF RANKS FOR PREFERENTIAL MATCHING Number of Assignable Probability (one-tailed) thaC the Indicated Sum of Ranks or Less Would occur by Chance - Ranks (N) 0,201 0.10 0.05 0.04 0.025 0,01 0.005 0.002 TO.001 0.0005 10-4 1 5 10-6 10-7 4 7 6 5 5 5 4 4 5 11 10 9 8 8 7 6 6 5 5 6 16 15 13 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 7 22 20 18 is 17 15 14 12 12 11 9 8 8 29 27 24 24 22 20 19 17 16 15 13 11 9 8 9 37 34 31 30 29 26 24 22 21 20 17 14 12 10 to 46 42 39 38 36 33 31 29 27 25 22 19 16 13 11 56 51 48 47 45 41 38 36 34 32 28 24 20 17 12 - 67 61 58 56 54 49 47 43 41 39 35 30 25 22 T VJt:. AILIN tuviv appaus omy to tnose speclai cases in wnicii tne number of occasions on which objects are being ranked (n) is equal to the number of assignable ranks (N). Each entry represents the largest number that is significant at the indicated p-level. Source: R. L. Morris [55 1. shielding does not prevent high-quality descriptions from being obtained. As a backup judging procedure, a panel of five additional SRI scientists not otherwise associated with the research were asked simply to blind match the unedited typed transcripts (with associated drawings) generated by the remote viewer against the nine target locations which they independently visited in turn. The transcripts were unlabeled and presented in random order. A correct match consisted of a transcript of a given date being matched to the target of that date. In- stead of the expected number of I match each per judge, the number of correct matches obtained by the five judges was 7, 6, 5, 3, and 3, respectively. Thus, rather than the expected total number of 5 correct matches from the judges, 24 such matches were obtained- B. Subject S4: Learner This experiment was designed to be a replication of our pre- vious experiment with Price, the first replication attempted. The subject for this experiment was Mrs. Hella Hammid, a gifted professional photographer. She was selected for this series on the basis of her successful performance as a per- cipient in the EEG experiment described earlier. Outside of that interaction, she had no previous experience with apparent paranormal functioning. At the time we began working with Mrs. Harnmid, she had no strong feelings about the likelihood of her ability to suc- ceed in this task. This was in contrast to both Ingo Swann who had come to our laboratory fresh from a lengthy and apparently successful series of experiments with Dr. Gertrude UNCLASSIFIED CPYRGHT i1w UNCLASSIFIED Am3roved For R~gpLs?c&OpaphlA~iogl&-AP~Wp~-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 337 PUTHOFF AND TARG: 'PERCEPTUAL CHA Wd PEDESTRIAN OVERPASS TARGET Q Fig. 4. Subject Hammid (S4) drawing, described as "some kind of diagonal trough up in the air." TABLE 11 DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS ASSIGNED To TRANSCRIPTS ASSOCIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION FOR ExPERIENCED SUBJECT PRICE (SI) Target Location Distance (km) Rank of Associated Transcript Hoover Tower, Stanford 3.4 1 Raylands Nature Preserve, Palo Alto 6.4 1 Radio tolescope@ Portola Valley 6.4 1 Marina, Redwood City 6.8 1 Bridge toll plaza, Fremont 14.5 6 Drive-in theater, Palo Alto 5.1 1 Arts and Craft$ Plaza, Menlo Park 1.9 1 Catholic Church, Portola Valley 8.5 3 swimming pool complex, Palo Alto 3.4 1 Total sum of ranks 16 (P=2. 9X10-1) who felt that he used his remote-viewing ability in his every- day life. In comparison with the latter two, many people are more influenced by their environment and are reluctant under public scrutiny to attempt activities that are generally thought to be impossible. Society often provides inhibition and nega- tive feedback to the individual who might otherwise have explored his own nonregular perceptual ability. We all share an historical tradition of "the stoning of prophets and the burning of witches" and, in more modern times, the hosp'italiza- tion of those who claim to perceive things that the majority do not admit to seeing. Therefore, in addition to maintaining scientific rigor, one of our primary tasks as researchers is to provide an environment in which the subject feels safe to explore the possibility of paranormal perception. With a new subject, we also try to stress the nominiqueness of the ability because from our experience paranormal functioning appears to be a latent ability that all subjects can articulate to some degree. Because of Mrs. Hammid's artistic background, she was ca- pable of drawing and describing visual images that she could not identify in any cognitive or analytic sense. When the target demarcation team went to a target location which was a pedestrian overpass, the subject said that she saw "a kind of trough up in th .e air," which she indicated in the upper part of her drawing in Fig. 4. She went on to explain, "If you stand where they are standing you will see something like this," indicating the nested squares at the bottom of Fig. 4. As it turned out, a judge standing where she indicated would have a view closely resembling what she had drawn, as can be seen from the accompanying photographs of the target loca- tion. It needs to be emphasized, however, that judges did not have access to our photographs of the site, used here for illustrative purposes only, but rather they proceeded to each of the target locations by list. In another experiment, the subject described seeing "an open barnlike structure with a pitched roof." She also saw a "kind of slatted side to the structure making light and dark bars on the wall." Her drawing and a photograph of the associated bicycle shed target are shown in Fig. 5. (Subjects are encouraged to make drawings of anything they visualize and associate with the remote location because drawings they make are in general more accurate than their verbal description.) As in the original series with Price, the results of the nine- UNCLASSIFIED CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED 59 Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 338 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 -7 Fig. 5. Subject Hammid (S4) response to bicycle shed target described as an open "barn-like building" with "slats on the sides" and a "pitched roof." TABLE III DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS ASSIGNED To TRANSCRIPTS ASSOCIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION FOR LEARNER SUBJECT HAMMID (S4) Target Location Distance (km) Rank of A::o,1:ted se 'pt Methodist Church, Palo Alto 1.9 1 Ness Auditorium, Menlo Park 0.2 1 Merry-go-round, Palo Alto 3.4 1 Parking garage, Mountain View 8.1 2 SRI International Courtyard, Menlo Park 0.2 1 Bicycle shed, Menlo Park 0.1 2 Railroad trestle bridge, Palo Alto 1.3 2 Pumpkin patch, Menlo Park 1.3 1 Pedestrian overpass, Palo Alto 5.0 2 Total sum of ranks 13 (p=I.8Xl0-6) experiment series were submitted for independent judging on a blind basis by an SRI research analyst not otherwise associ- ated with the research. While at each target location, visited in turn, the judge was required to blind rank order the nine unedited typed manuscripts of the tape-recorded narratives, along with any associated drawings generated by the remote viewer, on a scale I to 9 (best to worst match). The sum of ranks assigned to the target-associated transcripts in this case was 13, a result significant at p = 1.8 X 10-6 by exact calcula- tion (see Table I and discussion), and included five direct hits and four second ranks (Table III). ---------- Again, as a backup judging procedure, a panel of five addi- tional judges not otherwise associated with the research were asked simply to blind match the unedited typed tran- scripts and associated drawings generated by the remote viewer, against the nine target locations which they independently visited in turn. A correct match consisted of a transcript of a given date being matched to the target of that date.' In- stead of the expected number of I match each per judge, the number of correct matches obtained by the five judges was 5, 3, 3, 2, and 2, respectively. Thus, rather than the ex- pected total number of 5 correct matches from the judges, 15 such matches were obtained. C. Subjects S2 and S3: Experienced Having completed a series of 18 remote-viewing experiments, 9 each with experienced subject S 1 (Price) and learner S4 (Hammid), additional replication experiments, four with each subject, were carried out with experienced subjects S 2 (Elgin) and S 3 (Swann) and learners S 5 and S 6. To place the judging on a basis comparable to that used with S I and S4, the four transcripts each of experienced subjects S2 and S3 were com- bined into a group of eight for rank order judging to be com- pared with the similariy combined results of the learners S5 and S6. The series with S2 (Elgin, an SRI research analyst) provided afurther example of the dichotomy between verbal and draw- ing responses. (As with medical literature, case histories often are more illuminating than the summary of results.) The ex- periment described -here was -the -third -conducted with this UNCLASSIFIED BICYCLE SHED TARGET DETAIL OF BICYCLE SHED CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Amroved For-Release 2003LO911 0- -CIA--RDP-96-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 Fig. 6. Subject Elgin,(S2) drawings in response to tennis court target. so ow am Wo subject. It was a demonstration experiment for a government visitor who had heard of our work and wanted to evaluate our experimental protocol. In the laboratory, the subject, holding a bearing compass at arm's length, began the experiment by indicating the direction of the target demarcation team correctly to within So. (In all four experiments with this subject, he has always been within 100 of the correct direction in this angular assessment.) The subject then generated a 15-min tape-recorded description and the drawings shown in Fig. 6. In discussing the drawings, Elgin indicated that he was uncertain as to the action, but had the impression that the demarcation team was located at a museum (known to him) in a particular park. In fact, the target was a tennis court lo- cated in that park about 90 m from the indicated museum. Once again, we note the characteristic (discussed earlier) of a resemblance between the target site and certain gestalt ele- ments of the subject's response, especially in regard to the drawings, coupled with incomplete or erroneous analysis of the significances. Nonetheless, when rank ordering transcripts I through 8 at the site, the judge ranked this transcript as 2. This example illustrates a continuing observation that most of the correct information related to us by subjects is of a non- analytic nature pertaining to shape, form, color, and material rather than to function or name. A second example from this group, generated by S3 (Swann), indicates the level of proficiency that can be attained with practice. In the two years since we first started working with Swann, he has been studying the problem of separating the ex- ternal signal from the internal noise. In our most recent experiments, he dictates two lists for us to record. One list contains objects that he "sees," but does not think are located at the remote scene. A second list contains objects that he thinks are at the scene. In our evaluation, he has made much progress in this most essential ability to separate memory and imagination from paranormal. inputs. This is the key to bringing the remote-viewing channel to fruition with regard to its potential usefulness. The quality of transcript that can be generated by this pro- cess is evident from the results of our most recent experiment with Swann. The target location chosen by the usual double- blind protocol was the Palo Alto City Hall. Swann described a tall building with vertical columns and "set in" windows. His sketch, together with the photograph of the site, is shown in Fig. 7. He said there was a fountain, "but I don't hear it." At the time the target team was at the City Hall during the experiment, the fountain was not running. He also made an effort to draw a replica of the designs in the pavement in front of the building, and correctly indicated the number of trees (four) in the sketch. For the entire series of eight, four each from S2 and S3, the numerical evaluation 'based on blind rank ordering of tran- scripts at each site was significant at p = 3.8 X 10-4 and in- cluded three direct hits and three second ranks for the target- associated transcripts (see Table IV). D. Subjects S5 and S6: Learners To complete the series, four experiments each were carried out with learner subjects SS and S6, a man and woman on the SRI professional staff. The results in this case, taken as a UNCLASSIFIED TARGET-TENNIS COURTS CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED ... Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 rKVUr1r11J1n%JZk Ur ir1r, irrh, MAKurl IV./o ,a WU 40% tw. Mkl&b LAW-kw tub 4L-4 1 44 ObA J-h" .4 1,*.t A,.X vto CA^ W404 @Im __77 Fig. 7. Subject Swann (S3) response to City Hall target. group, did not differ significantly from chance. For the series of eight (judged as a group of seven since one target came up twice, once for each subject), the numerical evaluation based on blind rank ordering of transcripts at each site was non- significant at p = 0.08, even though there were two direct hits and two second ranks out of the seven (see Table V). One of the direct hits, which occurred with subject S6 in her first experiment, provides an example of the "first-time effect" that has been rigorously explored and is well-known to experi- menters in the field [57]. The outbound experimenter obtained, by random protocol from the pool, a target blind to the experimenter with the subject, as is our standard pro- cedure, and proceeded to the location. The subject, a math!e- matician in the computer science laboratory who had no pre- TABLE IV DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS ASSIGNED To TRANSCRIPTS ASSOCIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION FOR EXPERIENCED SUBJECTS ELGIN (S2) AND SWANN (S3) .Subject Target Location Distance (km) Rank of Associated Transcript S2 BART Station (Transit System), Fremont 16.1 1 S2 Shielded room, SRI, Menlo Park 0.1 2 S2 Tennis court@ Palo Alto 3.4 2 S2 Golf course bridge, Stanford 3.4 2 S3 City Hall, Palo Alto 2.0 1 S3 Miniature golf course@ Menlo Park 3.0 1 S3 Kioak in park, Menlo Park 0.3 3 S3 Baylands Nature Preserve, Palo Alto 6.4 3 Total sum of ranks 15 (p=3.&10@4) TABLE V DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS ASSIGNED To TRANSCRIPTS ASSOCIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION FOR LEARNER SUBJECTS S5 AND S6 Subiect Target Location Distance (km) Rank of Associated Transcript S5 Pedestrian overpass, Palo Alto 5.0 3 85 Railroad trestle bridge, Palo Alto 1.3 6 S5 WinJmill, portola Valley 8.5 2 S5p S6 White Plaza, Stanford (2) 3.8 1 S6 Airport, Palo Alto 5.5 2 S6 Kioak in Park, Menlo Park 0.3 5 S6 Boathouse, Stanford 4.0 1 Total am of ranks 20 (p.0.08, NS) vious experience in remote viewing, began to describe a large square with a fountain. Four minutes into the experiment, she recognized the location and correctly identified it by name (see Fig. 8). (It should be noted that in the area from which the target locations were drawn there are other fountains as well, some of which were in the target pool.) As an ex- ample of the style of the narratives generated during remote viewing with inexperienced subjects and of the part played by the experimenter remaining with the subject in such a case, we have included the entire unedited text of this experiment as Appendix B. E. Normal and Paranormal: Use of Unselected Subjects in Remote Viewing After more than a year of following the experimental pro- tocol described above and observing that even inexperienced subjects generated results better than expected, we initiated a series of experiments to explore further whether individuals other than putative "psychics" can demonstrate the remote- viewing ability. To test this idea, we have a continuing pro- gram to carry out additional experiments of the outdoor type with new subjects whom we have no a priori reason to believe have paranormal perceptual ability. To date we have collected data from five experiments with two individuals in this cate- gory: a man and a woman who were visiting government scientists interested in observing our experimental protocols. The motivation for these particular experiments was twofold. First, the experiments provide data that indicate the level of proficiency that can be expected from unselected volunteers. Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED IR gron 13 Aw CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED AppErRocved F -RQ?p%-00788R001 300070003-9 EPTJ @r&MeF;%01WpcWJR,I@AT PUTHOFF AND TARG. A T 341 Fig. 8. Subject (S6) drawing of White Plaza, Stanford University. Sub- ject drew what she called "curvy benches" and then announced cor- rectly that the place was "White Plaza at Stanford." 2/41Z A, 4L P@@ X A; d&. _1Y/K t@ Second, when an individual observes a successful demonstra- tion experiment involving an other person as subject, it inevi- tably occurs to him that perhaps chicanery is involved. We have found the most effective way to settle this issue for the observer is to have the individual himself act as a subject so as to obtain personal experience against which our reported results can be evaluated. The first visitor (VI) was invited to participate as a subject in a three-experiment series. All three experiments contained elements descriptive of the associated target locations; the quality of response increased with practice. The third re- RESPONSES OF VISITING sponse is shown in Fig. 9, where again the pattern elements in the drawing appeared to be a closer match than the subject's SCIENTIST SUBJECT analytic interpretation of the target object as a cupola. Fig. 9. Subject (V1) drawing of merry-go-round target. Am3roved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 CPYR UNCLASSIFIED GkpTproved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 342 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 TECHNOLOGY SERIES TYPEWRITER TARGET SUBJECT SWANN (S3) RESPONSE Fig. 10. Drawings of a typewriter target by two subjects. The second visitor [V21 participated as a subject in two ex- periments. In his first experiment, he generated' one of the higher signal-to-noise results we have observed. He began his narrative, "There is a red A-frame building and next to it is a large yellow thing [a tree-Editor]. Now further left there is another A-shape. It looks like a swing-set, but it is pushed down in a gully so I can't see the swings." [All cor- rect.] He then went on to describe a lock on the front door that he said "looks like it's made of laminated steel, so it must be a Master lock." [Also correct.] For the series of five-three from the first subject and two from the second-the numerical evaluation based on blind rank ordering of the transcripts at each site was significant at p = 0.0 17 and included three direct hits and one second - rank for the target-associated transcripts. (See Table VI.) TABLE VI DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS ASSIGNED To TRANSCRIPTS ASSOCIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION FOR VISITOR SUBJECTS VI AND V2 Subject Target Location Distance (km) Rank of Associated, Transcript V1 Bridge over stream, Menlo Park 0.3 1 V1 Baylands Nature Preserve, Palo Alto 6.4 2 V1 Merry-go-round, Palo Alto 3.4 1 V2 Windmill, Portola Valley 8.5 1 V2 Apartment swimming pool, Mountain View 9.1 3 Total sum of ranks (p.0.017) UNCLASSIFIED SUBJECT HAMMID (S4) RESPONSE its 9fK W,4 W*%k$4 4 t"k, &*Q.X QPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 PUTHOFF AND TARG: PPRCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER Will 343 TARGET LOCATION: XEROX MACHINE (TECHNOLOGY SERIES) TO ADD INTEREST TO TARGET LOCATION EXPERIMENTER WITH HIS HEAD BEING XEROXED C1, Fig. 11. Drawings by three subjects (S2, S3, and V3) for Xerox machine target. When asked to describe the square at upper left of response on the right, subject (V3) said, "There was this predominant light source which might have been a window, and a working surface which might have been the sill, or a working surface or desk." Earlier the subject had said, "I have the feeling that there is something silhouetted against the window." Observations with unselected subjects such as those de- scribed above indicate that remote viewing may be a latent and widely distributed perceptual ability. F. Technology Series: Short-Range Remote Viewing Comparisons of the targets and subject drawings for three of the multiple-response cases (the typewriter, Xerox machine, and video terminal) are shown in Figs. 10, 11, and 12. As is apparent from these illustrations alone, the experiments provide circumstantial evidence for an information channel of useful bit rate. This includes experiments in which visit- ing government scientists participated as subjects (Xerox machine and video terminal) to observe the protocol. In general, it appears that use of multiple-subject responses to a single target provides better signal-to-noise ratio than target identification by a single individual. This conclusion is borne out by the judging described below. Given that in general the drawings constitute the most accurate portion of a subject's description, in the first judging procedure a judge was asked simply to blind match only the drawings (i.e., without tape transcripts) to the targets. Multiple- subject responses to a given target were stapled together, and thus seven subject-drawing response packets were to be matched to the seven different targets for which drawings were made. The judge did not have access to our photographs of the target locations, used for illustration purposes only, but rather proceeded to each of the target locations by list. While standing at each target location, the judge was required to rank 6rder the seven subject- drawing response packets (presented in random order) on a scale I to 7 (best to worst match). For seven targets, the sum of ranks could range from 7 to 49. The sum in this case, which included I direct hit and 4 second ranks out of the 7 (see Table VII) was 18, a result significant at p = 0.036. In the second more detailed effort at evaluation, a visiting scientist selected at random one of the 12 data packages (a drill press experiment), sight unseen and submitted it for in- dependent analysis to an engineer with a request for an esti- Because remote viewing is a perceptual ability, we consid- ered it important to obtain data on its resolution capabilities. To accomplish this, we turned to the use of indoor techno- logical targets. Twelve experiments were carried out with five different sub- jects, two of whom were visiting government scientists. They were told that one of the experimenters would be sent by random protocol to a laboratory within the SRI complex and that he would interact with the equipment or apparatus at that location. It was further explained that the experimenter remaining with the subject was, as usual, kept ignorant of the contents of the target pool to prevent cueing during question- ing. (Unknown to subjects, targets in the pool were used with replacement; one of the goals of this particular experiment was to obtain multiple responses to a given target to investigate whether correlation of a number of subject responses would provide enhancement of the signal-to-noise ratio.) The sub- ject was asked to describe the target both verbally (tape recorded) and by means of drawings during a time-synchronized 15-min interval in which the outbound experimenter inter- acted in an appropriate manner with the equipment in the target area. In the twelve experiments, seven targets were used: a drill press, Xerox machine, video terminal, chart recorder, four- state random number generator, machine shop, and type- writer. Three of these were used twice (drill press, video terminal, and typewriter) and one (Xerox machine) came up three times in our random selection procedure. Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED UNCLASSIFIED CP.YRGHT Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 TARGET: VIDEO MONITOR FOR TEXT EDITING (TECHNOLOGY SERIES) Fig. 12 ,.61 rl 5-(,, (a) (b) Drawing by two subjects of a video monitor target. (a) Subject (S4) drawing of "box with light coming out of it painted flat black and in the middle of the room." (b) Second subject (V2) saw a computer terminal with relay racks in the background. TABLE VII DISTRIBUTION OF RANKINGS ASSIGNED TO SUBJECT DRAWINGS ASSOCIATED WITH EACH TARGET LOCATION Subject Target Rank of Associated Drawing. S3, S4 Drill press 2 S2, S3, V3 Xerox machine 2 S4, V2 Video terminal 1 S3 Chart recorder 2 S4 Random number generator 6 S4 Machine shop 3 S3, S4 Typewriter 2 Total am of ranks 18 (p=0.036) mate as to what was being described, The analyst, blind as to the target and given only the subject's taped narrative and drawing (Fig. 13), was able, from the subject's description alone, to correctly classify the target as a "man-sized vertical boring machine." G. Summary of Remote Viewing Results 8cussion 1) Di The descriptions supplied by the subjects in the experiments involving remote viewing of natural targets or laboratory apparatus, although containing inaccuracies, were sufficiently accurate to permit the judges to differentiate among various targets to the degree indicated. A summary TABLE VIII SUMMARY: REMOTE VIEWING Subject Number of Experiments p-Value, Rank Order Judging With natural targets S1 (experienced) 9 2.9 x 10-5 S2 and S3 (experienced) 8 3.8 x 10- 4 S4 (learner) 9 1.8 x 10-6 S5 and S6 (learners) 8 0.08 (NS) Vl and V2 (learners/visitors) 5 0.017 With technology targets S2, S3, S4, V2, V3 12 0.036 tabulation of the statistical evaluations of these fifty-one ex- periments with nine subjects is presented in Table VIII. The overall result, evaluated conservatively on the basis of a judging procedure that ignores transcript quality beyond that necessary to rank order the data packets (vastly underestimat- ing the statistical significance of individual descriptions), clearly indicates the presence of an information channel of useful bit rate, Furthermore, it appears that the principal difference between experienced subjects and inexperienced volunteers is not that the latter never exhibit the faculty, but rather that their results are simply less reliable, more sporadic. cribed earlier, Nevertheless, as des individual transcripts from the inexperienced group of subjects number among some of the best obtained. Such observations indicate a hypothesis that remote viewing may be a latent and widely distributed perceptual ability. Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED A proved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 PUTHOFF AND TARY PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER 345 Fig. 13. Subject (S4) drawing of drill press showing belt drive, stool, and a "vertical graph that goes up and down." Thus the primary achievement of the SRI program was the elicitation of high-quality remote viewing from individuals who agreed to act as subjects. Criticism of this claim could in principle be put forward on the basis of three potential flaws. 1) The study could involve naivet6 in protocol that permits various forms of cueing, intentional or unintentional. 2) The experiments discussed could be selected out of a larger pool of experiments of which many are of poorer quality. 3) Data for the reported experiments could be edited to show only the matching elements, the nonmatching elements being discarded. All three criticisms, however, are invalid. First, with regard to cueing, the use of double-blind protocols ensures that none of the persons in contact with the subject can be aware of the target. Second, selection of experiments for reporting did not take place; every experiment was entered as performed on a master log and is included in the statistical evaluations. Third, data associated with a given experiment remain unedited; all experiments are tape recorded and all data are included un- edited in the data package to be judged and evaluated. In the process of j u dging -attempting to match transcripts against targets on the basis of the information in the transcripts-some patterns and regularities in the transcript descriptions became evident, particularly regarding individual styles in remote viewing and in the perceptual form of the descriptions given by the subjects. These patterns and the judging procedure are discussed below. a) Styles of response: The fifty-one transcripts were taken from nine different subjects. Comparing the tran- scripts of one subject with those of another revealed that each pattern tended to focus on certain aspects of the remote target complex and to exclude others, so that each had an individual pattern of response, like a signature. Subject S3, for example, frequently responded with topo- graphical descriptions, maps, and architectural features of the target locations. Subject S2 often focused on the behavior of the remote experimenter or the sequence of actions he carried out at the target. The transcripts of subject S4, more than those of other subjects, had descriptions of the feel of the lo- cation, and experiential or sensory gestalts-for example, light/dark elements in the scene and indoor/outdoor and enclosed/open distinctions. Prominent features of Sl's tran- scripts were detailed descriptions of what the target persons were concretely experiencing, seeing, or doing-for example, standing on asphalty blacktop overlooking water; looking at a purple iris. The range of any individual subject's responses was wide. Anyone might draw a map or describe the mood of the remote experimenter, but the consistency of each subject's overall approach suggests that just as individual descriptions of a directly viewed scene would differ, so these differences also occur in remote-viewing processes. b) Nature of the description: The concrete descriptions that appear most commonly in transcripts are at the level of subunits of the overall scene. For example, when the target was a. Xerox copy machine, the responses included (S2) a rolling object (the moving light) or dials and a cover that is lifted (S3), but the machine as a whole was not identified by name or function. In a few transcripts, the subjects correctly identified and named the target. In the case of a computer terminal, the subject (V2) apparently perceived the terminal and the relay racks behind it. In the case of targets which were Hoover Tower and White Plaza, the subjects (S I and S6, respectively) seemed to identify the locations through analysis of their initial images of the elements of the target. There were also occasional incorrect identifications. Gestalts were incorrectly named; for example, swimming pools in a park were identified as water storage tanks at a water filtration plant (S 1). The most common perceptual level was thus an intermediate one-the individual elements and items that make up thetar- get. This is suggestive of a scanning process that takes sample perceptions from within the overall environment. When the subjects tried to make sense out of these fragmen- tary impressions, they often resorted to metaphors or con- structed an image with a kind of perceptual inference. From a feeling of the target as an "august" and "solemn" building, a subject (S4) said it might be a library; it was a church. A pedestrian overpass above a freeway was described as a conduit (S4). A rapid transit station, elevated above the countryside, was associated with an observatory (S2). These responses seem to be the result of attempts to process partial informa- UNCLASSIFIED CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 346 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 tion: similarly, this occurs in other parapsychological experi- ments. These observations are compatible with the hypotheses that information received in a putative remote-viewing mode is processed piecemeal in pattern form (consistent with a low bit rate process, but not necessarily requiring it); and the errors arise in the processes of attempted integration of the data into larger patterns directed toward verbal labeling. When the subjects augmented the verbal transcripts with drawings or sketches, these often expressed the target elements more accurately than the verbal descriptions. Thus the draw- ings tended to correspond to the targets more clearly and precisely than the words of the transcript. The descriptions given by the subjects sometimes went be- yond what the remote experimenter experienced, at least con- sciously. For example, one subject (S4) described and drew a belt drive at the top of a drill press that was invisible even to the remote experimenter who was operating the machine; another subject (S 1) described a number of items behind shrubbery and thus not visible to members of the demarcation team at the site. Curiously, objects in motion at the remote site were rarely mentioned in the transcript. For example, trains crossing the railroad trestle target were not described, though the remote experimenter stood very close to them. Also in a few cases, the subject descriptions were inaccurate regarding size of structures. A 20-ft courtyard separating two buildings was described as 200 ft wide, and a small shed was expanded to a barn-like structure. c) Blind judging of transcripts: The judging procedure entailed examining the transcripts for a given experimental series and attempting to match the transcripts with the cor- rect targets on the basis of their correspondences. The tran- scripts varied from coherent and accurate descriptions to mix- tures of correspondences and noncorrespondences. Since the judge did not know a priori which elements of the descriptions were correct or incorrect, the task was complicated, and tran- scripts often seemed plausibly to match more than one target. A confounding factor in these studies is that some target lo- cations have similarities that seem alike at some level of per- ception. For example, a radio telescope at the top of a hill, the observation deck of a tower, and a jetty on the edge of a bay all match a transcript description of "looking out over a long distance." A lake, a fountain, and a creek may all result in an image of water for the subject. Therefore, in several cases, even correct images may not help in the conservative differential matching procedure used. According to the judge, the most successful procedure was a careful elem ent-by- element comparison that tested each tran- script against every target and used the transcript descriptions and drawings as arguments for or against assigning the tran- script to a particular target. In most cases, this resulted in either a clear conclusion or at least a ranking of probable matches; these matches were subjected to the statistical analyses presented in this paper. 2) Summary: In summary, we do not yet have an under- standing of the nature of the inform ation-b earing signal that a subject perceives during remote viewing. The subjects com- monly report that they perceive the signal visually as though they were looking at the object or place from a position in its immediate neighborhood. * Furthermore, the subjects' per- ceptual viewpoint has mobility in that they can shift their point of view so as to describe elements of a scene that would not be visible to an observer merely standing at ground level and describing what he sees. (In particular, a subject often correctly describes elements not visible to the target demarca- tion team.) Finally, motion is seldom reported; in fact, moving objects often are unseen even when nearby static objects are correctly identified. A comparison of the results of remote viewing (a so-called free-response task) with results of forced-choice tasks, such as the selection of one of four choices generated by a random number generator [ 5 8 ], reveals the following findings. From a statistical viewpoint, a subject is more likely to describe, with sufficient accuracy to permit blind matching, a remote site chosen at random than he is to select correctly one of four random numbers. Our experience with these phenomena leads us to consider that this difference in task performance may stem from fundamental signal-to-noise considerations. Two principal sources of noise in the system apparently are mem- ory and imagination, both of which can give rise to mental pictures of greater clarity than the target to be perceived. In the random number task, a subject can create a perfect mental picture of each of the four possible outputs in his own imagi- nation and then attempt to obtain the correct answer by a mental matching operation. The same is true for card guessing experiments. On the other hand, the subject in remote view- ing is apparently more likely to approach the task with a blank mind as he attempts to perceive pictorial information from remote locations about which he may have no stored mental data. Finally, we observe that most of the correct information that subjects relate to us is of a nonanalytic nature pertaining to shape, form, color, and material rather than to function or name. In consultation with Dr. Robert Ornstein of the Langley- Porter Neuropsychiatric Institute, San Francisco, CA, and with Dr. Ralph Kiernan of the Department of Neurology, Stanford University Medical Center, Stanford, CA, we have formed the tentative hypothesis that paranormal functioning may involve specialization characteristic of the brain's right hemi- sphere. This possibility is derived from a variety of evidence from clinical and neurosurgical sources which indicate that the two hemispheres of the human brain are specialized for dif- ferent cognitive functions. The left hemisphere is predomi- nantly active in verbal and other analytical functioning and the right hemisphere predominates in spatial and other holistic processing [591, [60]. Further research is necessary to elucidate the relationship between right hemisphere function and paranormal abilities. Nonetheless, we can say at this point that the remote-viewing results of the group of subjects at SRI have characteristics in common with more familiar performances that require right hemispheric function. The similarities include the highly schematicized drawings of ob- jects in a room or of remote scenes. Verbal identification of these drawings is often highly inaccurate and the drawings themselves are frequently left-right reversed relative to the target configuration. Further, written material generally is not cognized. These characteristics have been seen in left brain-injured patients and in callosal-sectioned patients. As a result of the above considerations, we have learned to urge our subjects simply to describe what they see as opposed to what they think they are looking at. We have learned that their unanalyzed perceptions are almost always a better guide to the true target than their interpretations of the perceived data. Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 PUTHOFF AND TARG: PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER in, Will 347 IV. CONSIDERATIONS CONCERNING TIME If the authors may be forgiven a personal note, we wish to express that this section deals with observations that we have been reluctant to publish because of their striking apparent in- compatibility with existing concepts. The motivating factor for presenting the data at this time is the ethical consideration that theorists endeavoring to develop models for paranormal functioning should be apprised of all the observable data if their efforts to arrive at a comprehensive and correct descrip- tion are to be successful. During the course of the experimentation in remote viewing (Section III), subjects occasionally volunteered the informa- tion that they had been thinking about their forthcoming par- ticipation in a remote-viewing experiment and had an image come to them as to what the target location was to be. On these occasions, the information was given only to the experi- menter remaining at SRI with the subject and was unknown to the outbound experimenter until completion of the experi- ment. Two of these contributions were among the most accurate descriptions turned in during those experiments. Since the target location had not yet been selected when the subject communicated his perceptions about the target, we found the data difficult to contend with. We offer these spontaneous occurrences not as proof of pre- cognitive perception, but rather as the motivation that led us to do further work in this field. On the basis of this firsthand evidence, together with the copious literature describing years of precognition experiments carried out in various other labo- ratories, we decided to determine whether a subject could per- form a perceptual task that required both spatial and temporal remote viewing. It is well known and recently has been widely discussed that nothing in the fundamental laws of physics forbids the appar- ent transmission of information from the future to the present (discussed further in Section V). Furthermore, there is a gen- eral dictum that "in physical law, everything that is not forbid- den, is required" [61 ]. With this in mind, we set out to con- duct very well-controlled experiments to determine whether we could deliberately design and execute experiments for the sole purpose of observing precognition under laboratory conditions. The experimental protocol was identical to that followed in previous remote-viewing experiments with but one exception. The exception was that the subject was required to describe the remote location during a 15-min period beginning 20 min before the target was selected and 35 min before the outbound experimenter was to arrive at the target location. In detail, as shown in Table IX, each day at ten o'clock one of the experimenters would leave SRI with a stack of ten sealed envelopes from a larger pool and randomized daily, con- taining traveling instructions that had been prepared, but that were unknown to the two experimenters remaining with the subject. The subject for this experiment was Hella Hammid (S4) who participated in the nine-experiment series replicating the original Price work described earlier. The traveling experi- menter was to drive continuously from 10: 00 until 10: 30 be- fore selecting his destination with a random number generator. (The motivation for continuous motion was our observation that objects and persons in rapid motion are not generally seen in the remote-viewing mode of perception, and we wished the traveler to be a poor target until he reached his target site.) At the end of 30 min of driving, the traveling experimenter gener- TABLE IX ExPERIMENTAL PROTOCOL: PRECOGNITIvE REMOTE VIEWING Time Schedule Experi.enter/Subject Activity 10:00 Outbound experimenter leaves with 10 envelopes (containing target locations) and random number generator; begins half-hour drive 10:10 Experimenters remaining with subject in the laboratory elicit from subject a description of where outbound experimenter will be from 10:45-11:00 10:25 Subjec t response completed, at which time laboratory part of experiment is over 10:30 Outbound experimenter obtains random number from a random number generator, counts down to associated envelope, and proceeds to target location indicated tO:45 Outbound experimenter remains at target location for 15 minutes (10:45-11:00) ated a random digit from 0 to 9 with a Texas Instruments SR-51 random number generator; while still in motion, he counted down that number of envelopes and proceeded di- rectly to the target location so as to arrive there by 10:45. He remained at the target site until 11:00, at which time he re- turned to the laboratory, showed his chosen target name to a security guard, and entered the experimental room. During the same period, the protocol in the laboratory was as follows. At 10: 10, the subject was asked to begin a descrip- tion of the place to which the experimenter would go 35 min hence. The subject then generated a tape-recorded description and associated drawings from 10:10 to 10:25, at which time her part in the experiment was ended. Her description was thus entirely concluded 5 min before the beginning of the tar- get selection procedure. Four such experiments were carried out. Each of them ap- peared to be successful, an evaluation later verified in blind judging without error by three judges. We will briefly sum- marize the four experiments below. The first target, the Palo Alto Yacht Harbor, consisted en- tirely of mud flats because of an extremely low tide (see Fig. 14). Appropriately, the entire transcript of the subject per- tained to "some kind of congealing tar, or maybe an area of condensed lava. It looks like the whole area is covered with some kind of wrinkled elephant skin that has oozed out to fill up some kind of boundaries where (the outbound experi- menter) is standing." Because of the lack of water, the dock where the remote experimenter was standing was in fact rest- ing directly on the mud. Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED Fig. 14. Subject Hammid (S4) described "some kind of congealing tar, or maybe an area of condensed lava that has oozed out to fill up some kind of boundaries." CPYR9HT UNCLASSIFIED Iq pproved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 348 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 Note that the subject has learned not to rush into interpreta- tion as to the nature or purpose of the place. This is a result of our cautioning based on the observation that such efforts tend to be purely analytical and in our experience are almost invariably incorrect. If a subject can limit himself to what he sees, he is often then able to describe a scene with sufficient accuracy that an observer can perform the analysis for him and identify the place. The second target visited was the fountain at one end of a large formal garden at Stanford University Hospital (Fig. 15). The subject gave a lengthy description of a formal garden be- hind a wall with a "double colonnade" and "very well mani- cured." When we later took the subject to the location, she was herself taken aback to find the double colonnaded wall leading into the garden just as described. The third target was a children's swing at a small park 4.6 km from the laboratory (Fig. 16). The subject repeated again and again that the main focus of attention at the site was a "black iron triangle that the outbound experimenter had somehow walked into or was standing on." The triangle was "bigger than a man," and she heard a "squeak, squeak, about once a second," which we observe is a match to the black metal swing that did squeak. Fig. 17. Subject (S4) described a very tall structure located among city streets and covered with "Tiffany-like glass." The final target was the Palo Alto City Hall (Fig. 17). The subject described a very, very tall structure covered with "Tiffany-like glass." She had it located among city streets and with little cubes at the base. The building is glass-covered, and the little cubes are a good match to the small elevator exit buildings located in the plaza in front of the building. To obtain a numerical evaluation of the accuracy of the pre- cognitive viewing, the experimental results were subjected to independent judging on a blind basis by three SRI scientists who were not otherwise associated with the experiment. The judges were asked to match the four locations, which they visited, against the unedited typed manuscripts of the tape- recorded narratives, along with the drawings generated by the remote viewer. The.transcripts were presented unlabeled and in random order and were to be used without replacement. A correct match required that the transcript of a given experi- ment be matched with the target of that experiment. All three judges independently matched the target data to the response data without error. Under the null hypothesis (no information channel and a random selection of descriptions without re- placement), each judge independently obtained a result signifi- cant at p = (4!)-' = 0.042. For reasons we do not as yet understand, the four transcripts generated in the precognition experiment show exceptional co- herence and accuracy as evidenced by the fact that all of the judges were able to match successfully all of the transcripts to UNCLASSIFIED, Fig. IS. Subject (S4) described a formal garden "very well manicured" behind a double colonnade. Fig. 16. Subject (S4) saw a "black iron triangle that Hal had somehow walked into" and heard a "squeak, squeak, about once a second." CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED A proved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 PUTHOFF AND TART PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER 349 the corresponding target locations. A long-range experimental program devoted to the clarification of these issues and involv- Ing a number of subjects is under way. The above four experi- ments are the first four carried out under this program. Currently, we have no precise model of this spatial and tem- poral remote-viewing phenomenon. However, models of the universe involving higher order synchronicity or correlation have been proposed by the physicist Pauli and the psychologist Carl Jung [ 62 1. ACAUSALITY. If natural laws were an absolute truth, then of course there could not possibly be any processes that deviate from it. But since causalitys is a statistical truth, it holds good only on average and thus leaves room for exceptions which must somehow be experienceable, that is to say, real. I try to regard synchronistic events as acausal exceptions of this kind. They prove to be relatively independent of space and time; they rela- tivize space and time insofar as space presents in principle no ob- stacle to their passage and the sequence of events in time is in- verted so that it looks as if an event which has not yet occurred were causing a perception in the present. We shall see in the next section that such, a description, though poetic, has some basis in modern physical theory. V. DiscUSSION It is important to note at the outset that many contempo- rary physicists are of the view that the phenomena that we have been discussing are not at all inconsistent with the framework of physics as currently understood. In this emerg- ing view, the often-held belief that observations of this type are incompatible with known laws in principle is erroneous, such a concept being based on the naive realism prevalent before the development of modern quantum theory and information theory. One hypothesis, put forward by 1. M. Kogan of the USSR, is that information transfer under conditions of sensory shielding is mediated by extremely low-frequency (ELF) electromagnetic waves in the 300-1000-km region [37]_ [40]. Experimental support for the hypothesis is claimed on the basis of slower than inverse square attenuation, com- patible with source-percipient distances lying in the induc- tion field range as opposed to the radiation field range; ob- served low bit rates (0.005-0.1 bit/s) compatible with the information carrying capacity of ELF waves; apparent ineffec- tiveness of ordinary electromagnetic shielding as an attenuator; and standard antenna calculations entailing biologically gener- ated currents yielding results compatible with observed signal- to-noise ratios. M. Persinger, Psychophysiology Laboratory, Laurentian Uni- versity, Toronto, Canada, has narrowed the ELF hypothesis to the suggestion that the 7.8-Hz "Shumann waves" and their harmonies propagating along the earth-ionosphere waveguide duct may be responsible. Such an hypothesis is compatible with driving by brain-wave currents and leads to certain other hypotheses such as asymmetry between east-west and west- east propagation, preferred experimental times (midnight-4 A.m.), and expected negative correlation between. success and the U index (a measure of geomagnetic disturbance throughout the world). Persinger claims initial support for these factors on the basis of a literature search [ 63 ] , [ 64 ]. , On the negative side with regard to a straightforward ELF interpretation as a blanket hypothesis are the following: a) ap- As usually understood. parent real-time descriptions of remote activities in sufficient detail to require a channel capacity in all probability greater than that allowed by a conventional modulation of an ELF signal; b) lack of a proposed mechanism for coding and decod- ing the information onto the proposed ELF carrier; and c) ap- parent precognition data. The hypothesis must nonetheless re- main open at this stage of research, since it is conceivable that counterindication a) may eventually be circumvented on the basis that the apparent high bit rate results from a mixture of low bit rate input and high bit rate "filling in the blanks" from imagination; counterindication b) is common to a number of normal perceptual tasks and may therefore simply reflect a lack of sophistication on our part with regard to perceptual functioning. [65]; and counterindication c) may be accom- modated by an ELF hypothesis if advanced waves as well as retarded waves are admitted [66], [671. Experimentation to determine whether the ELF hypothesis is viable can be carried out by the use of ELF sources as targets, by the study of para- metric dependence on propagational directions and diurnal timing, and by the exploration of interference effects caused by creation of a high-intensity ELF environment during ex- perimentation, all of which are under consideration in our lab- oratory and elsewhere. Some physicists believe that the reconciliation of observed paranormal functioning with modern theory may take place at a more fundamental level-namely, at the level of the founda- tions of quantum theory. There is a continuing dialog, for example, on the proper interpretation of the effect of an ob- server (consciousness) on experimental measurement [681, and there is considerable current interest in the implications for our notions of ordering in time and space brought on by the observation [691, [761 of nonlocal correlation or "quan- tum interconnectedness" (to use Bohm's term [711) of distant parts of quantum systems of macroscopic dimensions. The latter, Bell's theorem [721, emphasizes that "no theory of reality compatible with quantum theory can require spatially separated events to be independent" [731, but must permit interconnectedness of distant events in a manner that is con- trary to ordinary experience [741-[751. This prediction has been experimentally tested and confirmed in the recent experiments of, for example, Freedman and Clauser [691, [701. E. H. Walker and 0. Costa de Beauregard, independently proposing theories of paranormal functioning based on quan- tum concepts, argue that observer effects open the door to the possibility of nontrivial coupling between consciousness and the environment and that the nonlocality principle permits such coupling to transcend spatial and temporal barriers [761, [771. Apparent "time reversibility"-that is, effects (e.g., observa- tions) apparently preceding causes (e.g., events)-though con- ceptually difficult at first glance, may be the easiest of appar- ent paranormal phenomena to assimilate within the current theoretical structure of our world view. In addition to the familiar retarded potential solutions f(t - r1c), it is well known that the equations of, for example, the electromagnetic field admit of advanced potential solutions f(t + r/c)-solutions that would appear to imply 4 reversal of cause and effect. Such solutions are @conventionally discarded as not corresponding to any observable physi* event. One is cautioned, however, by statements such as that of Stratton in his basic text on electro- magnetic theory [ 7 8 1. Approved For Relea U09/10: CIA-RDPE660788 ROO 1300070003-9 DRLLASSII CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED 350 The reader has doubtless noted that the choice of the function f(t - r1c) is highly arbitrary, since the field equation admits also a solution f(t + r1c). This function leads obviously to an advanced time, implying that the field can be observed before it has been generated by the source. The familiar chain of cause and effect is thus reversed and this alternative solution might be discarded as logically inconceivable. However, the application of "logical" causality principles offers very insecure footing in matters such as these and we shall do better to restrict the theory to retarded actidin solely on the grounds that this solution alone conforms to the present physical data. Such caution is justified by the example in the early 1920's of Dirac's development of the mathematical description of the relativistic electron that also yielded a pair of solutions, one of which was discarded as inapplicable until the discovery of the positron in 1932. In an analysis by 0. Costa de Beauregard, an argument is put forward that advanced potentials constitute a convergence toward "finality" in a manner symmetrical to the divergence of retarded potentials as a result of causality [771. Such phenomena are generally unobservable, however, on the gross macroscopic scale for statistical reasons. This is codified in the thermodynamic concept that for an isolated system entropy (disorder) on the average increases. It is just this requirement of isolation, however, that has been weakened by the observer problem in quantum theory, and 0. Costa de Beauregard argues that the finality principle is maximally operative in just those situations where the intrusion of consciousness as an ordering phenomenon results in a significant local reversal of entropy increase. At this point, further discussion of the subtleties of such considerations, though apropos, would take us far afield, so we simply note that such advanced waves, if detected, could in certain cases constitute a carripr of information precognitive to the event. The above arguments are not intended to indicate that the precise nature of the information channel coupling remote events and human perception is understood. Rather, we in- tend to show only that modern theory is not without resources that can be brought to bear on the problems at hand, and we expect that these problems will, with further work, continue to yield to analysis and specification- Furthermore, independent of the mechanisms that may be involved in remote sensing, observation of the phenomenon implies the existence of an information channel in the inform ation-the ore tic sense. Since such channels are amenable to analysis on the basis of communication theory techniques, as indicated earlier, channel characteristics such as bit rate can be determined independent of a well-defined physical channel model in the sense that thermodynamic concepts can be ap- plied to the analysis of systems independent of underlying mechanisms. Furthermore, as we have seen from the work of RyzI discussed in Section 11, it is possible to use such a channel for error-free transmission of information if redundancy coding is used. (See also Appendix A.) Therefore, experimentation involving the collection of data under specified conditions Per- mits headway to be made despite the formidable work that needs to be done to clarify the underlying bases of the phenomena. VI, CONCLUSION For the past three years we have had a program in the Elec- tronics and Bioengineering Laboratory of SRI to investigate those facets of human perception that appear to fall outside the range of well-understood perceptual or processinit cara- PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 bilities. The primary achievement of this program has been the elicitation of high-quality "remote viewing"-the ability of both experienced subjects and inexperienced volunteers to view, by means of innate mental processes, remote geo- graphical or technical targets such as roads, buildings, and laboratory apparatus. Our accumulated data from over fifty experiments with more than a half-dozen subjects indicate the following. a) The phenomenon is not a sensitive function of distance over a range of several kilometers. b) Faraday cage shielding does not appear to degrade the quality or accuracy of perception. c) Most of the correct information that subjects relate is of a nonanalytic nature pertaining to shape, form, color, and material rather than to function or name. (This aspect suggests a hypothesis that information transmission under conditions of sensory shielding may be mediated pri- marily by the brain's right hemisphere.) d) The principal difference between experienced subjects and inexperienced volunteers is not that the latter never exhibit the faculty, but rather that their results are simply less reliable. (This observa- tion suggests the hypothesis that remote viewing may be a latent and widely distributed, though repressed, perceptual ability.) Although the precise nature of the information channel cou- pling remote events and human perception is not yet under- stood, certain concepts in information theory, quantum theory, and neurophysiological research appear to bear directly on the issue. As a result, the working assumption among re- searchers in the field is that the phenomenon of interest is consistent with modern scientific thought, and can therefore be expected to yield to the scientific method. Further, it is recognized that communication theory provides powerful techniques, such as the use of redundancy coding to improve signal-to-noise ratio, which can be employed to pursue special- purpose application of the remote-sensing channel independent of an understanding of the underlying mechanisms. We there- fore consider it important to continue data collection and to encourage others to do likewise; investigations such as those reported here need replication and extension under as wide a variety of rigorously controlled conditions as possible. APPENDix A SIGNAL ENHANCEMENT IN A PARANORMAL COMMUNICATION CHANNEL By APPLICATION OF REDU14DANCY CODING Independent of the mechanisms that may be involved in remote sensing, observation of the phenomenon implies the existence of an information channel in the information- theoretic sense. As we have seen from the work of Ryzl dis- cussed in Section 11,6 it is even possible to use such a (noisy) channel for error-free transmission of infonnation if suf- ficient redundancy coding is used [ 30], [ 3 11. Following is a general procedure that we have used successfully for signal enhancement. We shall assume that the "message" consists of a stream of binary digits (0,1) of equal probability (e.g., binary sort of green/white cards as in Ryzl's case, English text encoded as in Table X and sent long distance by strobe light on/off, and so on). To combat channel noise, each binary digit to be sent through the channel requires the addition of redundancy bits (coding). Efficient coding requires a compromise between the desire to maximize reliability and the desire to minimize re- 6 See also the note added in proof on the successful work done by Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED___ hm CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003109110 : CIA-RDP96-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 PUTHOFF AND TARG: PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER TABLE X 5-BIT CODE FOR ALPHANUMERIC CHARACTERS E 00000 Y 01000 T 11111 G'J 10111 N 00001 W 01001 R 11110 V 10110 1 00010 B 01010 0 11101 0 10101 A 00011 1 01011 S,X,Z 11100 2 10100 D 00100 3 01100 H 11011 4 10011 L 00101 5 01101 c,K,Q 11010 6 10010 F 00110 7 01110 P 11001 8 10001 U 00111 9 01111 X L 11000 10000 Note: Alphabet characters listed in order of decreasing frequency in English text. See, for example, A. Sinkov [791. Crhe low-fre- quency letters, X, Z, K, Q, and J, have been grouped'with similar characters to provide space for numerics in a .5-bit code.) In consideration of the uneven dis- tribution of letter frequencies in English text, this code is chosen such that 0 and I have equal probability. dundancy. One efficient coding scheme for such a channel is obtained by application of a sequential sampling procedure of the type used in production-line quality control [801. The adaptation of such a procedure to paranormal communication channels, which we now discuss, was considered first by Taetzsch [ 811. The sequential method gives a rule of proce- dure for making one of three possible decisions following the receipt of each bit: accept 1 as the bit being transmitted; reject I as the bit being transmitted (i.e., accept 0); or continue transmission of the bit under consideration. The sequential sampling procedure differs from fixed-length coding in that the number of bits required to reach a final decision on a message bit is not fixed before transmission, but depends on the results accumulated with each transmission. The principal advantage of the sequential sampling procedure as compared with the other methods is that, on the average, fewer bits per final decision are requ .ired for an equivalent degree of reliability, Use of the sequential sampling procedure requires the speci- fication of parameters that are determined on the basis of the following considerations. Assume that a message bit (0 or 1) is being transmitted. In the absence ofa priori knowledge, we may assume equal probability (p = 0.5) for the two possibili- ties (0, 1). Therefore, from the standpoint of the receiver, the probability of correctly identifying the bit being transmitted is p = 0.5 because of chance alone. An operative remote-sensing channel could then be expected to alter the probability of correct identification to a value p = 0.5 + @, where the param- eter 0 satisfies 0 < I V1 I < 0.5. (The quantity may be positive or negative depending on whether the paranormal channel results in so-called psi-hitting or psi-missing.) Good psi func- tioning on a repetitive task has been observed to result in V/ = 0.12, as reported by RyzI [31]. Therefore, to indicate the design procedure, let us assume a baseline psi parameter Vlb = 0. 1 and design a communication system on this basis. NC 60 0 0 < z (n w 'm 0=) 40 L) Lu 2W 0 30 LL < Q @J X W LU 20 W 2 :) UJ Z 0 cc 10 LU < 2 351 - DECISION 1 Accept "'I" as - the Bit Being ECISION 3 Transmitted Continue - ransmissio DECISION 2 Accept "0" as the Bit Being Transmitted 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 NUMBER OF TRIALS Fig. 18. Enhancement of signal-to-noise ratio by sequential sampling procedure (po = 0.4, p I = 0.6, a = 0.0 1, A = 0.0 1). The question to be addressed is whether, after repeated transmission, a given message bit is labeled a "I" at a low rate po commensurate with the hypothesis Ho that the bit in ques- tion is a "0," or at a higher rate p, commensurate with the hypothesis HI that the bit in question is indeed a "l." The decision-making process requires the specification of four parameters. po The probability of labeling incorrectly a "0" message bit as a " 1. " The probability of labeling correctly a "0" as a "0" is P = 0-5 + Ob = 0.6. Therefore, the probability of labeling incorrectly a "0" as a "I" is I - p = 0.4 = po. p, The probability of labeling correctly a "I" message bit as a "l," is given by p, = 0.5 + ob = 0.6. a The probability of rejecting a correct identification for a "0" (Type I error). We shall take C1 = 0.01. The probability of accepting an incorrect identification for a "l" (Type 11 error). We shall take 0= 0.01. With the parameters thus specified, the sequential sampling procedure provides for construction of a decision graph as shown in Fig. 18. The equations for the upper and lower limit lines are =dI +SN -do + SN where log - ce log d, El- I - Po do - Pi I - po log log P0 I - Pi PO I - pi I - po log I - pl- S = - log pi I - Po po I - Pi in which S is the slope, N is the number of trials, and d, and do are the y-axis intercepts. A cumulative record of receiver- generated responses to the target bit is compiled until either IFIED CPYRGHT 352 UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 1.0 8E 0.8 0 > 0.6 0 M ' V1 a) 0.4 >.E L: UP @c in- -E 0.2 < E _J 'T LU cu cc 'D 0 Fig. 19. Reliability curve for sequential sampling procedure (p, 0.4, p, =0.69ot=0.01@0=0.01). the upper or the lower limit line is reached, at which point a decision is made to accept 0 or I as the bit being transmitted. Channel reliability (probability of correctly determining message being transmitted) as a function of operative psi parameter 0 is plotted in Fig. 19. As observed, the sequential sampling procedure can result in 90 percent or greater reliability with psi parameters on the order of a few percent. Implementation of the sequential sampling procedure, re- quires the transmission of a message coded in binary digits. Therefore, the target space must consist of dichotomous ele- ments such as the white and green'cards used in the experi- ments by Ryzl. In operation, a sequence corresponding to the target bit (0 or 1) is sent and the cumulative entries are made (Fig. 18) until a decision is reached to accept either a I or a 0 as the bit being transmitted. At a prearranged time, the next sequence is begun and continues as above until the entire message has been received. A useful alternative, which relieves the percipient of the burden of being aware of his self-contradiction from trial to trial, consists of cycling through the entire message repeti- tively and entering each response on its associated graph until a decision has been reached on all message bits. The authors have used this technique successfully in a pilot study, but a discussion of this would take us beyond the intended scope of this paper. From the results obtained in such experiments, the channel bit rate can be ascertained for the system configuration under consideration. Furthermore, bit rates for other degrees of reliability (i.e., for other po, p 1, ce, and 0) can be estimated by construction of other decision curves over the same data base and thus provide a measure of the bit rate per degree of In summary, the procedures described here can provide for a pecification of the characteristics of a remote-sensing channel inder well-defined conditions. These procedures also provide or a determination of the feasibility of such a channel for Particular applications. APPENDIx B REMOTE-VIEWING TRANSCRIPT Following is the unedited transcript of the first experiment ith an SRI volunteer (S6), a mathematician in the computer ience laboratory, with no previous experience in remote viewing. The target, determined by random procedure, was White's Plaza, a plaza with fountain at Stanford University (shown in Fig. 8). As is our standard protocol, the experi- menter with the subject is kept ignorant of the specific target visited as well as the contents of the target pool. The experi- menter's statements and questions are italics. Today is Monday, October 7th. It is 11: 00 and this is a re- mote viewing experiment with Russ Targ, Phyllis Cole, and Hal Puthoff. In this experiment Hal will drive to a remote site chosen by a random'process. Phyllis Cole will be the remote viewer, and Russ Targ is the monitor. We expect this experi- ment to start at twenty minutes after eleven and run for fif- teen minutes. It is just about twenty minutes after eleven and Hal should be at his target location by now. Why don't you tell me what kind of pictures you see and what you think he might be doing or experiencing. The first thing that came to mind was some sort of a large, square kind of a shape. Like Hal was in front of it. It was a ... not a building or something, it was a square. I don't know if it was a window, but something like that so that the bottom line of it was not at the ground. About where his waist was, at least. That's what it seemed to me. It seems outdoors some- how. Tree. Does Hal seem to be looking at that square? I don't know. The first impression was that he wasn't, but I have a sense that whatever it was was something one might look at. I don't know if it would be a sign, but something that one might took at. Can you tell if it is on the ground or vertical? It seemed vertical. I don't have a sense that it was part of anything particular. It might be on a building or part of a building, but I don't know. There was a tree outside, but I also got the impression of cement. I don't have the impression of very many people or traffic either. I have the sense that he is sort of walking back and forth, I don't have any more explicit picture than that. Can you move into where he is standing and try to see what he is looking at? I picked up he was touching something-something rough. Maybe warm and rough. Something possibly like cement. It is twenty-four minutes after eleven. Can you change your point of view and move above the scene so you can get a bigger picture of what's there? I still see some trees and some sort of pavement or some- thing like that. Might be a courtyard. The thing that came to mind was it might be one of the plazas at Stanford campus or something like that, cement. Some kinds of landscaping. I said Stanford campus when I started to see some things in White Plaza, but I think that is misleading. I have the sense that he's not moving around too much. That it's in a small area. I guess I'll go ahead and say it, but I'm afraid I'm just putting on my impressions from Stanford campus. I had the impres- sion of a fountain. There are two in the plaza, and it seemed that Hal was possibly near the, what they call Mem Claw. What is that? It's a fountain that looks rather like a claw. It's a black sculpture. And it has benches around it made of cement. Aye there any buildings at the place you are looking at? Are there any buildings? You described a kind of a courowd. Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED +0.4 +0.2 0 -0.2 -0.4 0 (psi parameter) aw CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Am3roved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP9piOO788ROO1300070003-9 PUTHOFF AND TARa: 'PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSF 353 Usually at some places there should be a building, la or rg@ 18,1974. Look at the end or the sides ut d i b ll h h t [5) D. D. Home, Lights and Shadows of Spiritualism. New York: s a o . sma e cour t at t yar G. W. Carleton, 1877. of the courtyard. Is there anything to be seen? (61 J. Coover, Experiments in Psychical Research. Palo Alto, CA: 1 have a sense that there are buildings. It's not solid build [7) Stanford Univ. Pr@ss, 1917. G. Estabrooks, Bull. Boston Society for Psychical Research, 1927. ings. I mean there are some around the periphery and I have a See also [ 12, pp. 18 -19 J. sense that none of them are very tall. Maybe mostly one story, 18] L. T. Troland, Techniques for the Experimental Study of Telep- one ion l two stor b athy and OtherAlleged Clairvoyant Processes. Albany, NY, 1928. . e an occas a may y 191 J. B. Rhine, New Frontiers of the Mind. New York: Farrar and Do you have any better idea of what your square was that Rinehart, 1937. you saw at the outset? 110] J. Pratt and J. B. Rhine et al., Extra-Sensory Perception after Sixty Years. New York: Henry Holt, 1940. No. I could hazard different kinds of guesses. [11) C. Scott, "G. Spencer Brown and probability: A critique," Does it seem part of this scene? J. Soc. PsychkalRes., vol. 39, pp. 217-234,1958. It could almost be a bulletin board I think it could be It [121 G. R. Price, "Science and the supernatural," Science, vol. 122, . ... pp.359-367,1955. or something with notices on it maybe. [13] -, "Apology to Rhine and Soal," Science, vol. 175, p. 359, Or something that people are expected to look at. Maybe a 1972. " " window with things in it that people were expected to look at. [141 J. A new case of experimenter unreliability, J. B. Rhine, Parapsychol., vol. 38, pp. 215-225, June 1974. What kind of trees do you see in this place? 1151 S. G. Soal and F. Bateman, Modern Experiments in Telepathy. I don't know what kind they are. The impression was that London, England: Faber and Faber, 195.3. "' ' they were shade trees and not terribly big. Maybe 12 feet of [16] explanation of the Soal- C Scott and P. Haskell, Normal Goldney experiments in extra-sensory perception," Nature, vol. trunk and then a certain amount of branches above that. So 245, pp. 52-54, Sept. 7, 1973. that the branches have maybe a 12 foot diameter, or some- [171 C. E. M. Hansel, ESP-A Scientific Evaluation. New York: Scribner, 1966. thing. Not real big trees. (181 J. B. Rhine and J. G. Pratt, "A review of the Pearce-Pratt dis- New trees rather than old trees? tance series of ESP tests," J. Parapsychol., vol. 18, pp, 165-177, Yeah, maybe 5 or 10 years old, but not real old ones. [191 1954. J. G. Pratt and J. L. Woodruff, "Size of stimulus symbols in extra- Is there anything interesting about the pavement? sensory perception," J. Parapsychol., vol. 3, pp. 121-158, 1939. No. It seems to be not terribly new or terribly old. Not [201 5. G. Soal and H. T. Bowden, The Mind Readers: Recent Expert- very interesting. There seems to be some bits of landscaping [21] mentsin Telepathy. New Haven, CT: Yale Univ. Press, 1954. C. Honorton, "Error some placel" J. Commun., vol. 25, no. I around. Little patches of grass around the edges and periph- (Annenberg School of Commun.), Winter 197 S. not lush. eries. Maybe some flowers. But [221 M. Ryzl, "Training the psi faculty by hypnosis," J. Amer. Soc. , Psychical Res., vol. 41, pp. 234-251, 1962. You saw some benches. Doyou want to tell me about them? [231 CIBA Foundation Symposium on Extra Sensory Perception. Well, that's my unsure feeling about this fountain. There Boston, MA: Little, Brown, 1956. it felt Curved benches ome kind of benches of cement w [241 M. RyzI and J. Pratt, "A repeated-calling ESP test with sealed " . as s , cards, J. Parapsychol., vol. 27, pp. 161-174,1963. like. [25] -, "A further confirmation of stabilized ESP performance in a They were of rough cement. selected subject,".1. Parapsychol., vol. 27, pp. 73-83, 1963. " ' ' [26] ESP sub- J. Pratt, Preliminary experiments with a borrowed What do you think Hal is doing while he is there? ject," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 42, pp. 333-345, 1964. 1 have a sense that he is looking at things tTying to project 1271 J. Pratt and J. Blom, "A confirmatory experiment with 'bor- Looking at different things and sort of walking back them rowed" outstanding ESP subject," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., . vol. 42, pp. 381-388, 1964. and forth not covering a whole lot of territory. [281 W. G. Roll and J. G. Pratt, "An ESP test with aluminum targets," Sometimes standing still while he looks around. J. Amer. Soc. PsychicalRes., vol. 62, pp. 381-387,1968. and I almost sense re n of him talkin t h d th i i I j [291 J. Pratt, "A decade of research with a selected ESP subject: An " us e ss o a mp g, overview and reappraisal of the work with Pavel Stepanek, that it was being recorded or something. I don't know if he Proc. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 30, 197 3. but if it's not that, then he is saying some- has a tape recorder [301 C. Shannon and W. Weaver, The Mathematical Theory of Com- , thing because it needed to be remembered. It's 11:33. He's [31] munication. Urbana, IL: Univ. Illinois Press, 1949. M. Ryzi, "A model for parapsychological communication," just probably getting ready to come back. J. Parapsychol., vol. 30, pp. 18-31, Mar. 1966. [32] C. Tart, "Card guessing tests: Learning paradigm or extinction paradigm,." J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 60, p. 46, 1966. ACKNOWLEDGMENT [33] M. Ullman and S. Kripprier, with A. Vaughan, Dream Telepathy. The authors wish to thank the principal subjects, Mrs. Hella [341 New York@ Macmillan, 1973. C. Honorton, "State of awareness factors in psi activation," J. Hammid, Pat Price, and Ingo Swann, who showed patience and Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 68, pp. 246-2 57, 1974. forbearance in addition to their enthusiasm and outstanding [351 Proc. 2nd Int. Congr. Pvychotronk Research (Monte Carlo). perceptual abilities. We note with sadness the death of one of [361 Cotati, CA: Int. Assoc. Psychotronic Res., 1975. L. L. Vasiliev, Experiments in Mental Suggestion. Hampshire, our subjects, Mr. Price. We express our sincere thanks also to England: ISMI Publ., 1963. Bonnar Cox, and Dr. Arthur Hastings, of SRI, and Earle Jones [371 1. M. Kogan, "Is telepathy possible?" Radio Eng., vol. 2 1, p. 7 5, , Mrs. Judith Skutch and Richard Bac1l, without whose en- [381 Jan.1966. -, "Telepathy, hypotheses and observations," Radio Eng., vol. couragement and support this work could not have taken 22, p. 141, Jan. 1967. place [391 -, "Information theory analysis of telepathic communication " . experiments, Radio Eng., vol. 23, p. 122, Mar. 1968. [40] -, "The information theory aspect of telepathy," RAND Publ., REFERENCES Santa Monica, CA, p. 4145, July 1969. [41] A. S. Presman, Electromagnetic Fields and Life. New York: [1) J. R. Smythies, Ed., Science and ESP, London, England: Rout- Plenum, 1970. ledge, 1967. [421 Y. A. Kholodov, Ed., Influence of Magnetic Fields on Biological [21 C. Evans, "Parapsychology -What the questionnaire revealed," Objects, JPRS 63038, NTIS, Springfield, VA, Sept. 24, 1974. New Scientist, Jan. 25, 1973, p.,209. [43] Y. A. Kholodov, "Investigation of the direct effect of magnetic [3] A. Gould., The Founders of Psychical Research. New York: fields on the central nervous system," in Proc. Ist Conf. Psycho- Schocken Books, 1968. See also W. Crookes, Researches in the tronic Res., JPRS L/5022-1 and 2, Sept. 6, 1974. Phenomena of Spiritualism. London, England: J. Burns, 1874. 144) D. Mennie, "Consumer electronics," IEEE Spectrum, vol. 12, 14] R. Targ and H. Puthoff, "Information transmission under condi- pp. 34-35, Mar. 1975. tions of sensory shielding," Nature, vol. 252, pp. 602-607, Oct. [451 W. P. Zinchenko, A. N. Leontiev, B. M. Lomov, and A. R. Luria, Approved For Relea 3/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 DRCLASSIFIED CPYRGHT UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 354 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, MARCH 1976 [461 [471 [481 149] 150] [511 [521 [531 [541 [551 [56] [57] [58] 1591 [601 [61] [621 [631 "Parapsychology: Fiction or reality?" Questions ofPhilosophy, vol. 9, pp. 128-136,1973. R. Cavanna, Ed., Proc. Int. Conf. Methodology In PSI Research. New York: Parapsychology Foundation, 1970. E. D. Dean, "Plethysmograph recordings as ESP responses," Int. J. Neuropsychiatry, vol. 2, Sept. 1966. C. Tart, "Physiological correlates of psi cognition," Int. J. Para- psychol., no. 4, 1963. D. H. Lloyd, "Objective events in the brain correlating with psychic phenomena," New Horizons, vol. 1, no. 2, Summer 1973. J. Silverman and M. S. Buchsbaum, "Perceptual correlates of con- sciousness; A conceptual model and its technical implications for psi research," in Psi Favorable States of Consciousness, R. Ca- vanna, Ed. New York: Parapsychology Foundation, pp. 143- 169, 1970. J. Kamiya, "Comment to Silverman and Buchsbaum," Ibid., pp. 158-159. D. Hill and G. Parr, Electroencephalography. A Symposium on Its Various Aspects. New York: Macmillan, 1963. T. D. Duane and T. Behrendt, "Extrasensory electroencephalo- graphic induction between Identical twins," Science, vol. 150, p.367,1965. K. Osis, ASPR Newsletter, no. 14, 1972. R. L. Morris, "An exact method for evaluating preferentially matched free-response material," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 66, p. 401, Oct. 1972. G. R. Schmeidler, "PK effects upon continuously recorded tem- peratures," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 67, no. 4, Oct. 1973. W. Scherer, "Spontaneity as a factor In ESP," J. Amer. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 12, pp. 12 6-147, 1948. R. Targ, P. Cole, and H. Puthoff, "Techniques to enhance man/ machine communication," SRI, Menlo Park, CA, Final Rep., NASA Contract NAS7-1 00, June 1974. R. Ornstein, The Nature of Human Consciousness. San Fran- cisco, CA: Freeman, 1973, ch. 7 and 8. R. W. Sperry, "Cerebral organization and behavior," Science, vol. 133, pp. 1749-1757, 1961. 0. Bilaniuk and E. C. G. Sudarshan, "Particles beyond the light barrier," Phys. Today, vol. 22, May 5, 1969. W. Pauli and C. G. Jung, Eds., The Interpretation of Nature and the Psyche (Bollingen Ser. LI). Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press, 1955. M. A. Persinger, "ELF waves and ESP," New Horizons Trans. Toronto Society for Psychical Research, vol. 1, no. 5, Jan. 1975. 164] -, "The paranormal-P. II: Mechanisms and models," M.S.S. Information Corp., New York, 1974. 165 ] B. Julesz, Foundations of Cyclopean Perception. Chic.ago, IL: Univ. Chicago Press, 197 1. [661 H. Puthoff and R. Targ, in Psychic Exploration -A Challenge for Science, J. White, Ed. New York: Putnam, 1974, pp. 522-542. [67] G. Feinberg, "Precognition -A memory of things future?" in Proc. Conf. Quantum Physics and Parapsychology (Geneva, Switzerland). New York: Parapsychology Foundation, 1975. 168] E. P. Wigner, "The problem of measurement," Amer. J. Phys., vol. 3 1, no. 1, p. 6, 1963. [69] J. J. Freedman and J. F. Clauser, "Experimental test of local hidden variable theories," Phys. Rev. Lett., vol. 28, no. 14, p. 938, Apr. 3, 1972. 1701 J. F. Clauser and M. A. Horne, "Experimental consequences of objective local theories," Phys. Rev. D, vol. 10, no. 2, p. 526, July 15, 1974. [711 D. Bohm and B. Hiley, '@On the intuitive understanding of non- locality as implied by quantum theory" (Birkbeck College, London, England), Feb. 1974, Preprint. [72 ] J. S. Bell, "On the problem oi hidden variables In quantum theory," Rev. Mod. Phys., vol. 38, no. 3, p. 447, July 1966. [731 H. Stapp, "Theory of reality," Lawrence-Berkeley Lab. Rep. LBL-3837, Univ. California, Berkeley, Apr. 1975. (74] A. Einstein, B. Podolsky, and N. Rosen, "Can quantum-mechani- cal description of physical reality be considered complete? " Phys. Rev., vol. 47, p. 777, May 15, 1935. [751 R. H. Dicke and J. P. Wittke, Introduction to Quantum Mechan- Ics. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, 1960, ch. 7. 1761 E. H. Walker, "Foundations of paraphysical and parapsychologi- cal phenomena," In Proc. Conf. Quantum Physics and Para- psychology (Geneva, Switzerland). New York; Parapsychology Foundation, 1975. (77] 0. Costa de Beauregard, "Time symmetry and interpretation of quantum mechanics," Lecture delivered at Boston Colloquim for Philosophy of Science (Feb. 1974), Foundations ofPhysics (in press). (781 J. A. Stratton, Electromagnetic Theory. New York; McGraw- Hill, 1941. [79] A. Sinkov, Elementary Cryptanalysts-A Mathematical Approach. New York: Random House, 1968. [801 P. Hoel, Introduction to Mathematical Statistics, 2nd ed. New York: Wiley, 1954, p. 27. [811 R. Taetzsch, "Design of a psi communications system," Int. J. Parapsychol., vol. 4, no. 1, p. 35, Winter 1962. Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED, Approved For Releau ffftA --c'AATrW788R001 300070003-9 55' TAB B CURRICULUM VITAE. (U) HAROLD E. PUTHOFF, Ph.D. RADIO PHYSICS LABORATORY SRI INTERNATIONAL UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 HAROLD E. PUTHOFF, Ph. D. Radio Physics Laboratory SRI International Menlo Park, California Dr. Harold E. Puthoff was born in Chicago, Illinois, on June 20, 1936. His undergraduate and beginning graduate programs were pursued at the University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida', from which he received the Bachelor of Electrical Engineering degree with High Honors in 1958, and the Master of Science in Engineering degree in 1960. While at the University of Florida he received an award for Outstanding Engineering Student., and was awarded memberships in several scholastic and scientific-honorary fraternities (Phi Eta Sigma, Phi Kappa Phi, Sigma Tau, and Sigma Xi). After graduating with a Master's degree from the University of Florida in 1960, he reported to active duty as a commissioned officer in the Navy and was@assigned a research billet at NSA, Ft. Meade,, MD. While there he pursued research and monitored contracts concerned with the development of ultra-high-speed computers under project LIGHTNING. He also initiated a program to determine the potential of lasers, fiber optics, and other devices.of ehis nature for use in optical and opto- electronic computers. His work in this area resulted in the award of a Department of.Defense Certificate of Commendation for outstanding Per- formance, and an award of Second Prize for Outstanding Paper of the Year,, NSA Technical Journal., 1963. Upon being released from active duty in February 1963, he r.emained in a civilian status (GS-13) until September 1963, at which time he entered Stanford University to pursue a Ph.D. program. Following receipt of his Ph.D. degree'from Stanford in 1967, he accepted positions as Research Associa@e at the Microwave Lab and Lecturer in the Department of Electrical Engineering at Stanford' where he pursued teaching and research in lasers and nonlinear optics. While at Stanford, he conceived, patented, and developed a tunable Raman Laser wnicn Ali produces high power radiation throughout the infrared portion of the spectrum. He also supervised research for Ph.D. candidates in Electrical Engineering and Applied Physics, published over 25 papers in professional journals on lasers and microwave devices, and published.a te xtbook on lasers (in English, French, and Russian) widely used in universities I Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED UNCLASSIFIE D Approved For Release 2003109110 : CIA-RDP96-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 both here and abroad'. Fundamentals of Quantum Electronics (Wiley, 1969). In 1972, he joined the staff of Stanford Research Institute (now SRI International) as a senior researcher where he initiated and is presently engaged in research in quantum physics and parapsychological phenomena. In the SRI work he has been investigating facets of human percep tion that appear to fall outside the range of well-understood perceptual/ processing capabilities. Of particular interest has been a human informa- tio.n-accessing capability,that the SRI team calls "remote viewing." This phenomenon pertains to the ability of certain individuals to access and describe, by means of'mental processes, information sources blocked from ordinary perception, and generally accepted as secure against such access. In particular, the phenomenon most extensively inves tigated is the ability of subJects to view remote geographical locations up to several thousand km distant from their physical location given only geographical.coordinates or a known person on whom to'target. The remote perceptual abilities of subjects have been developed sufficiently to allow them to describe-- oft en in great detail--geographical or technical material such as buildings, roads, and natural formations. The accumulated data indicate that both specially selected and unselected persons can be assisted in developing remote perceptual abiliti es up to a level of useful information tra.nsf er. His articles published in Nature (1974) and Proc. IEEE (1976) are the first major articles on the subject of paranormal phenomena to appear in general science journals. He has also co-authored a book Mind-Reach, wit h an Introduction by Margaret Mead (Delacorte PressY 1977), in which the first fiveyears of research at SRI were brought to the general public, and has in press a co-edited second book of collected technical papers on remote viewing., Mind at Large: Institute of Electrical and Electronic Engineers Symposia on the Natur e of Extrasensory Perceptio (Praeger, 1979). The general acceptance in the scientific community of his and his colleagues' work at SRI resulted in his being invited to keynote the 1976 IEEE Systems, Man and Cybernetics Society meeting in Washington, D.C., for which he was awarded the IEEE Franklyn V. Taylor Award for the best presentation at.the conference; resulted in an invitation to him to 2 Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCIASSIFIED UNCLASSIFIED - Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 participate in a Special Session on the State of the Art in Psychic Research at the IEEE's national convention in New York City (ELECTRO 177, April 1977); resulted in an invitation to him to keynote the 1977 IEEE Microwave Theory an d Techniques national conference in June 1977; and resulted in an invitation to him to chair a session in parapsychology and research at the IEEE Systems,, Man and Cybernetics meeting in Washington, D.C., September 1977. He presently serves in various.capacities as consultant and contractor to the DoD and Intelligence Communities in the parapsychological area. 3 Approved For Release 2003/09/10 CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-.9 PUBLICATIONS OF HAROLD E. PUTHOFF W.W. Peterson and H.E. Puthoff, "A Theoretical Study of Ion Plasma Oscillations IRE Elect. Dev., Vol. ED-6, p. 372 (1959). H.E. Puthoff , it Crossed-Field Focusing of a Hollow Cylindrical Electron Beam," U. of Florida M.S. Thesis (January 1960). A.D. Sutherland, H.F. Renkiewicz, H.E. Puthoff, and D.E. Countiss, tt On the Use of Periodic Electrostatic Focusing in Klystrons," presented at the International Congress on Microwave Tubes, Munich, West.Germany (June 1960). H.E. Puthoff , ?I Design of a Cros@ed-Field Electron Gun," presented at the Eighteenth Conference on Electron Tube Research, Seattle, Washington (June 1960). H.E. Puthoff Scaling Matrix for the Analog Computer," NSA Tech. Jour., Vol. 7 (1962). J.T. Tippett and H.E. Puthoff, "The Status of Optical Logic Elements for Nanosecond Computer Systems,." Proc. Pacific Computer Conf., Pasadena, California (March 196.3). Also published in,NSA Tech. Jour., Vol. 8 (1963). J.T. Tippett and H.E. Puthoff, "Optical Computers Approach Realit* yy Electronics, Vol. 36, p. 72 (May 1963). Also selected for publication in Optoelectronic Devices and Circuits, ed. by Samuel Weber,.p. 266 (McGraw- Hill Book Co., Now York, N.Y., 1964). R.H. Pantell and H.E. P uthoff, "A Proposed Novel Method for Obtaining a Non-Spiking Pulsed Laser IEEE Vol. 53, p. 295 (1965). R.H. Pantell, P. Pheneger, and H.E. Puthoff, Transient Buildup of Laser. Oscillations, I''presented at.Spring Meeting of Electrochemical Society, San Francisco, California (May 1965). B. Huth, R.H. Pantell and H.E. Puthoff Characteristics of a Tunable Raman Laser," presented at Spring Meeting of Electrochemical Society, San Francisco, California (May 1965). B. Huth, R. Pantell and H. Puthoff , The Effect of a Static Field on Raman Scattering," presented at 23rd Conference on Electron Device Research, Urbana, Illinois (June.1965). H.E. Puthoff, R.H. Pantell, and B.G. Huth , Tunability of the Raman taserl" J. Appl. Phys., Vol. 37, p. 860 (1966). R.H. Pantell, B.G. Huth, H.E. Puthoff, and R.L. Kohn , Mode Coupling in an External Raman Resonator," Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 9, p. 104 (1966). B.G. Huth, N.V. Karlov, R.H. Pantell and H.E. Puthoff , A Study of the log Stimulated Raman Effect Using an Off-Axis Resonator," presented at 24th Conferenceon Electron Device Research, Pasadena, California (June 1966). Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 VASSIFIED UNCLASSIFIED Approved For'Release 2003/09/10: CIA-RDP96 .-00788RO01 300070003-9 AN B.G. Huth, N.V. Karlov, R.H. Pantell, and H.E. Puthoff Characteristics.of the Stimulated Raman Effect in an External Resonator , Proc. Sixth Int, Conf. on Microwave and Optical Generation and Amplification, Cambridge, England (September 1966). mi B.G. Huth, N.V. Karlov, R.H. Pantell., and H.E. Puthoff, "A Quantitative Study of the Stimulated Raman Effect Using an Off-Axis Resonator tv 'Ell Jour. Quant. Elect.-, Vol. QE-2, p. 763 (December 1966). R. Pantell, F. Praderc, J. Hanus, and 11. Puthoff , Theoretical and Experimental Values for Two, Three' 'IV and Four Photon Absorptions Jour. Chem. Physics, Vol. 46, P. 3507 (1 967).. H.E. Puthoff, R.H. Pantell, B.G., Huth, and M.A. Chacon , Near-Forward Raman Scattering in LiNbO 31?r J. Appl. Phys., Vol. 39, p. 2144 (March 1968). It R.H. Pantell, B.G. Huth, and H.E. Puthoff , Generation of Nonthreshold If Second-Stokes Using an,Off-Axis Resonator , IEEE Jour. Quant. Elect. (Corres.), Vol. QE-4, p. 41 (January 1968). R.H@ Pantell, G. Soncini, and H.E. Puthoff , 11 Stimulated Photon-Electron Scattering," IEEE Jour. Quant. Elect., Vol. QE-4.1 p. 905 (1968). J. Gelbwachs, R.H. Pantell, H.E. Puthoff, and J.M. Yarborough , A Tunable Stimulated Raman Oscillator vt Appl. Phys. Letters Vol. 14j' p. 258 (1969). J.M. Yarborough, S.S. Sussman, H.E. Puthoff, R.H. Pantell and B.C ' Joh nson, It Efficient Tunable Optical Emission from LiNbO Without a Resonator , 3 Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 15, p. 102 (1969).. S.I. Way, M. Chodorow, and H.E. Puthoff,'"Optical Damage in KDP," Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 16, No.4 (1970). S.S. Sus sman, B.C. Johnson, J.M. Yarborough, H.E. Puth off, R.H. Pantelll ,, and J. Soo Hoo, "A New Source of.Tunable Optical and Infrared Radiation , Proc. Polytechnic Inst. of Brooklyn International Symposium on Submillimeter Waves, New York, N.Y. (March 31 to April 21 1970). B.C. Johnson, H.E. Puthoff, J. Soo Hool, and S.S. Sussman Power and Linewidth of Tunable Stimulated Far IR Emis -sion in LiNbO 3111 Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 18, p. 181 (1970). E. Amzallag, T.S. Chang, B.C. Johnson, R.H. Pantell and H.E. Puthoff, "Stimulated Raman and Polariton Scattering in L1103,IIJ._ Appl. Phys., Vol. 42, p. 3251 (1971). D'.L. Hecht, W.L. Bond, R.H. Pantell, and H.E. Puthoff , I? Dye Lasers with Ultrafast Transverse Flow 'it IEEE Jour. Quant. Elect., Vol. QE-8,, p. 15 (1972). H.E. Puthoff and R. Targ "Experiments with Uri Geller film for general release, Stanford Research Institute, Menlo Park, California (March 1973). 2 20 09/10 : C - Approved For Relea ON Ot LAO PRITUR001 300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 H.E. Puthoff,"Psychoenergetic Research at Stanford Research Institute, presented at lst International Congress on Parapsychology and Psycho- tronics, It Prague, Czech. (June 1973). mod; H.E. Puthoff and R. Targ, "PK Experiments.with Uri Geller and. IIngo Swann," Research in Parapsychology 1973 (Scarecrow Press, Inc... Metuchen., N.J..,, 1974). r Research in R. Targ and H.E. Puthoff ESP Experiments with Uri Gelle Parapsychology 1973, pp. 57-60 (Scarecrow Press, Inc., Metuchen, N.J., 1974). It M.A. Piestrup, R.H. Pantell, H.E. Puthoff, and G.B. Rothbart Cerenkov Radiation as a Source of Ultraviolet Radiation," Jour. Appl. Phys. . Vol. 44, . No. 11, p. 5160 (November 1973).' I H.E. Puthoff and R. Targ ''Remote Viewing of Natural Targets, Research in Parapsychology, 1974 (Scarecrow Press, Inc., Metuchen, N.J., 1975). R. Targ and 11. Puthoff , Remote Viewing of Natural Targets,".Proc. 23rd Annual International Conference, Quantum Physics and.Parapsychology, Geneva, Switzerland (August 1974). H.E. Puthoff and R. Targ, "Physics, Entropy, and Psychokinesis," Proc. 23rd Nei Annual International Conference, Quantum Physics and Parapsychology, Geneva, Switzerland (August 1974). H.E. Puthoff and R. Targ Psychic Research and Modern Physics,' , Psychic Exploration--A Challenge for Science,, John White., ed. (G.P. Putnam's Sons, New York, N.Y., p. 524, 1974). R. Targ and H.E. Puthoff , Information Transmission Under Conditions of -Sensory Shielding," Nature, Vol. 251, p. 602 (October 1974). R. Targ and H. Puthoff , Remote Viewing of Natural Targets , Parapsychology Review, Vol. 61 No. 1 (January-February 1975). H. Puthoff and R. Targ, "Precognitive Remote Viewing,' t Research in Para- psychology, 1975 (Scarecrow Press, Inc.,,Metuchen, N.J., 1976). R. Targ and 11. Puthoff , Replication Study of the Remote Viewing of Natural Targets , Research in Parapsychology, 1975 (Scarecrow Press, Inc., Metuchen, N.J., 1976). H.E. Puthoff and R. Targ "A Perceptual Channel for Information Transfer It over Kilometer Distances: Historical Perspective and Recent Research , Proc. IEEE, Vol. 64, pp. 329-354 (March 1976). H.E. Puthoff and R. Targ, "Direct Perception of Remote Geographical 'It XTRO '77 Special Session on the State of the Art Locations Proc. IEEE ELE in Psychic Research, New York, N.Y. (April 19-21, 1977). 3 Approved For, Releau WCU L_CUA_PffbY788R001300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED., Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 E.C. May, R. Targ, and II.E. Puthoff, "Possible L@EG Correlates to Remote Stimuli Under Conditions of Sensory Shielding,"Proceedings of the 'IEEE ELECTRO '77 Special Session on the State of the Art in Psychic Research, New York, N.Y. (April 19-21, 1977). AN R. Targ, H.E. Puthoff and E.C. May, "State of the Art in Remote Viewing Studies at SRI," Proceedings of the 1977 IEEE International Conference on Cybernetics and Society Technical Session on Research in Psychoenergetics, Washington, D.C. (September 20, 1977). H.E. Puthoff, R. Targ, and E. C. May, "Experimental Psi Research: Implications for Physics," Presented at the 145th National Meeting of the AAAS (American Association for the Advancement of Science), Houston, Texas (January 1979). IT.'E. Puthoff and R. Targ, ",Information Transmission in Remote Viewing Experiments: II," submitted to Nature. Books: R.H. Pantell and H.E. Puthoff, Fundamentals of Quantum Electronics (John Wiley and Sons, Inc., New York, 1969) [English]; (Dunod, Paris, 1973) [French]; (Mir Press, Moscow, 1972) [Russian]. R. Targ and H.E. Puthoff, Mind-Reach (Delacorte Press, New York, N.Y., 1977). C. Tart, H. Puthoff and R. Targ, Mind at Large: Institute of Electrical and Electronic Engineers Symposia on the Nature of.Extrasensory Perception (Praeger Press, New York, N.Y., in press). 4 Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300,070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED !.CIA no Approved For Relea fjftftAY5 ffrrff 788RO01 300070003-9 TAB C CURRICULUM VITAE (U) RUSSELL TARG RADIO PHYSICS LABORATORY SRI INTERNATIONAL UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO13000700.03-9 RUSSELL TARG Radio Physics Laboratory SRI International Menlo Park, California Russell Targ was born in Chicago, Illinois, April 11, 1934. He attended public schools in Chicago, Cleveland, and New York City, and obtained his B.S. degree in,physics from Queens College, at age 20, in 1954. He was awarded a research-assistantship in the Physics Department at Columbia University. He pursued graduate studies from 1954 to 1956 in the laboratory of Professor C.S. Wu. While in both college and graduate school, Mr. Targ partially supported himself as a stage magician and became familiar with the paraphernalia and misdirection used in professional magic. This experience led to his interest in the paranormaL In 1956 Mr. Targ left Graduate School to-work in the then new field of gas plasma research at the Sperry Gyroscope Company in Great Neck,, Long Island. He was with the Electronic Tube Division., where he did original work in the generation of high-power microwaves from electron beam formed plasmas, leading in 1958 to publication of a paper describing the first such deviceJ, and also initiating a continuing contractual support for this research area from the Air Force Cambridge Research Center. Mr. Targ also did early work in the technology of ultra-high vacuum. In 1958 he was invited to join the Polytechnic Institute of Brooklyn to become a Research Associate at the Microwave Research Laboratory and assist in the formation of the new electron beam laboratory. He performed experiments with high-density plasmas,, making use of microwave diagnostic techniques for the measurement of plasma density. In 1959, because of his past plasma research, he was asked by his Columbia University associates to join them in research at the newly-formed Technical Research Group, to develop the gas laser which had just been. proposed in the scientific.literature. Mr. Targ was involved in experi- ments with new gaseous laser media, spectroscopy with various systems of gasses, construction of He-Ne lasers in the visible and the infrared spectral ranges, and the first optical heterodyne experiments using phase modulation and demodulation techniques. Approved For Releasp 003 09/10 : CIA -RDP96-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 URLA 91FIED - UNCLASSIFIED, Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 In 1958 Mr. Targ married Joan Fischer, sister of Bobby Fischer, the World Chess Champion, and in 1962 the family moved to'Palo Alto, California, to seek a more congenial environment to raise children than could be found in New York City. Mr. Targ worked with GTE Sylvania in Mt.- View, Calif., from 1962 to 1972. Most recently, he investigated techniques for the development of new gas lasers, making use of his researc h on compact, self-contained, multikilowatt Co2 lasers. He has pursued research resulting from his original investigations of the FM laser and super-mode laser techniques. This work included frequency st-abilization and single frequency operation of both He-Ne lasers and high power argon lasers. Mr. Targ joined Stanford Research Institute in 1972 as a Senior Research Physicist to help establish a psychoenergetics research program. The purpose of this program has been to investigate a wide area of para- normal functioning, and attempt to bring it within the framework of modern physics. Since the initiation of this program, it has achieved recognition and encouragement from the engineering, scientific and government communities. Publications and Inventions Author or coauthor of more than thirty articles on lasers and plasma research., and more recently, the first major publication of research on psychoenergetic phenomena in Nature and in the Proceedings of the IEEE: "Information Transfer Under Conditions of Sensory Shielding," Nature (October 18, 1974) and "A Perceptual Channel for Information Transfer over Kilometer Distances," Proc. IEEE (March 1976). Coauthor of Mind-Reach: Scientists Look at Psychical Research (Delacorte Press, New York, 1976). Coeditor of Mind at Large: Institute of Electrical and Electronic Engineers Symposia on the Nature of Extrasensory Percepti (Praeger Press, New York, in press). ,Invention of the tunable plasma oscillator at microwave frequencies. Professional Associations and Honors aw aw Senior Member IEEE; American Physical Society Research associate in physics with the Polytechnic Institute of Brooklyn. 2 Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 UNCLASSIFIED. k.MCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788R00130007000-9 PUBLICATIONS OF RUSSELL TARG M. Ettenberg.and R. Targ, "Observations of Plasma and Cyclotron Oscillations , Proc. of the-Symposium on Electronic Waveguides, Polytechnic Institute of Brooklyn, New York (April 8-10, 1958). R. Targ and L.P. Levine , V1 Backward-Wave Oscillations in a System Composed of an Electron Beam and a Hydrogen Gas Plasma , J. of Appl. Phys., Vol. 32, No. 4, pp. 731-737 (April 1961). P. Rabinowitz, S. J.acobs, R. Targ, and G. Gould, "Honodyne Detection of Phase-Modulated Light," Proc. IRE, Vol. 50, No. 11 (November 1962). G. Grosof and R. Targ, ."Enhancement in Mercury-Krypton and Xenon-Krypton Gaseous Discharges 'it Appl-.--Opties, Vol. 2, No. 3,'pp. 299-302 (March 1963). R. Targ, "Optical Heterodyne Detection of Microwavo-Modulated Light@ Proc. IEEE (Correspondence), pp. 303-304 (March 1964). R. Targ, D.E. Caddes, and B.J. McMurtry, "The Traveling-Wave Phototube. Part II: Experimental Analysis , IEEE Trans. on Electron Devices, I Vol. ED-11, pp. 164-170 (April 1964). ow S.E. Harris.and R. Targ, "FM Oscillation of the He-Ne Laser,".App. Phys. Letters Vol. 5 No. 10, pp. 202-204 (15 November 1964). R..Targ, G.A. Massey, and S.E. Harris, "Laser Frequency Translation by . ti Means of Electro-Optic Coupling Control , Proc. IEEE (Correspondence), Vol. 52, No. 10, pp. 1247-1248 (October 1964). R. Targ and W.D. Bush, "Automatic Frequency Control of a Laser Local Oscillator for the Heterodyne Detection of Microwave-Modulated Light," Appl. Optics, Vol. 41 No. 11, pp. 523-527 (December 1965). G.A. Massey, M.K. Oshman, and R. Targ, "Generation of Single-Frequency Light Using the FM Laser 'It Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 61 No. 1, pp. 10-11 (January 1965). L.M. Osterink and R. Targ, Single-Frequency Light from an Argon FM Laser '11 Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 10, No. 4, pp. 115-117 (February 1967). R. Targ and J.M. French, "Stabilization of a He-Ne Laser," Proc. IEEE, Vol. 55, No. 7, pp. 1185-1192 (July 1967). L.M. Osterink and R. Targ, "Single-Frequency Light Using the Super-Mode ti Technique with an Argon FM Laser , Proc. of the Symposium on Modern Optics, Polytechnic Institute of Brooklyn, Now York (March 22-24, 1967). R. Targ and L.M. Osterink Frequency Stabilization and Quieting of the FM. 'if, SCON Convention Record, I Laser . 1967 WE San Francisco California (1967). Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 .@'NCLASSIFIED UNCLA. _JFI ED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 R. Targ and J.M. Yarborough Mode-Locked Quieting of the He-Ne and Argon Lasers it?, Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 121 No. 1, pp. 3-4 (1 January 1968). D.E. Caddes, L.M. 0 sterink, and R. Targ, "Mode-Locking of the C02 Laser," , Vol. 12, No.'3, pp. 74'76 (1 February 1968). Letters Phys. Appl. . . . R. Targ, J.M. Yarborough, and J.M. French, "Frequency Stabilization and Noise Suppression in the Argon FM Laser," IEEE J. of Quant. Elect., Vol. QE-4, pp. 644-648 (October 1968). W.B. Tiffany, R. Targ, and J.D. Foster, "Kilowatt CO 2 Gas-Transport Laser," Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 15, No. 3 (1969). W.B. Tiffany, and R. Targ, The Gas-Transport Laser--A New Class of High- Power Electro-Optic Devices 'If 11ser Focus, pp. 48-50 (September 1969). Bi R. Targ and W.B. Tiffany, "Gain and Saturation in Transverse Flowing CO 2-N 2-110 Mixtures, Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 15, No. 9 (1 November 1969). S.E. Schwarz, T.A. deTemple, and R. Targ, "High Pressure Pulsed Xenon ti Laser, Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 17, No. 7 (1 October 1970). J.D. Taynail R. Targ, and W.B. Tiffany, "An Investigation of Tellurium for Frequency Doubling with CO 2Lasers," IEEE J. of Quant. Elect., Vol. QE-7 (8 August 1971). R. Targ and M.W. Sasnett , 11 High Repetition Rate Xenon Laser with Transverse Excitation ?I IEEE J. of Quant. Elect., Vol. QE-8, pp. 166-169 (February 1972). R. Targ and M.W. Sasnett , it Xenon-Helium Laser at High Pressure and High Repetition Rate," Appl. Phys. Letters, Vol. 19, No. 12 (15 December 1971). R. Targ, "Pulsed Nitrogen Laser at High Repetition Rate," IEEE J. of Quant. Elect., Vol. QE-8, pp. 126-728 (August 1972). R. Targ @nd D. Hurt, "Learning Clairvoyance and Precognition with an Extra-' sensory Perception Teaching Machine," Parapsychology Review, pp. 9-11 (July 1972). T.S. Fahlen and R. Targ, "High-Average-Power Xenon Laser," IEEE Journal of Quantum Electronics, Vol . QE_9 I No. 6 (1973). H.E. Puthoff and R. Targ, "Physics, Entropy " and Psychokinesis , 23rd Intl. Conf., Quantum Physics and Parapsychology, Geneva (1974). it R. Targ and 11. Puthoff, Remote Viewing of Natural Targets," Proc. 23rd Intl. Conference, Quantum Physics and Parapsychology, Geneva (1974). H.E. Puthoff and R. Targ, Psychic Research and Modern Physics," Psychic Exploration--A Challenge to Science, John Whitel Ed. (Putnam and Sons, N.Y.) 1974). R. Targ and H. Puthoff , Information Transmission Under Conditions of Sensory Shielding," Nature, Vol. 251 (1974). 2 Approved For Release 0788ROO1300070003-9 _U ffelrx L D UNCLASSIFIED ad Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 11. E. PuLhoff and R. Targ, "A Perceptual Channel for Inforyrtation Transfer Over Kilometer Distances: Historical Perspective and Recent Research," Proceedings of the IEEE, Vol. 64, No. 3, pp. 329-354 (March 1976). R. Targ and H. E. PuthoffY Mind Reach: Scientists Look at Psychical Research (Delacorte Press., New York, N.Y.J. 1977). H. E. Puthoff and R. Targ, "Direct Perception of Remote Geographical Locations 'it Proceedings of the IEEE ELECTRO 177 Special Session on The State of the Art in Psychic Research, New York, N.Y., April 19-21, 1977. E. C. May, R. Targ, and H. E. Puthoff , Possible EEG Correlates to Remote Stimuli Under Conditions of Sensory Shielding," Proceedings of the IEEE ELECTRO '77 Special Session on the State of the Art in Psychic Research, New York, N.Y., April 19-21, 19t7. R. Targ, H. E. Puth off and E. C. May, "State of the Art in Remote Viewing Studies at SRI," Proceedings of the 1977 IEEE International Conference on Cybernetics and Society Technical Session on Research in Psychponergetics, Washington, D.C., September 20, 1977. C. Tart, H. Puthoff, and R. Targ, Mind at Large:. Institute of Electrical and Electronic Engineers Symposia on the.Nature of Extrasensory Perceptio (Praeger Press, New York, N.Y., in press). H.E. Puthoff, R. Targ, and E.C. May, "Experimental Psi Research: Implications for Physics," presented at the 145th National Meeting of the AAAS (American. Association for the Advancement of Science), Houston, Texas (January 1979). H.E. Puthoff and R.- Targ, "Information Transmission in Remote Viewing Experiments: II,11 submitted to Nature. 3 Approved For Release jQ0erX ttUW.SEff 788RO01 30 .0070003-9 U 1@ I I: Approved For Relea : -C LA 96-6788RO01 300070003-9 DREIASSME Im TAB D mm INFORMATION ABOUT REMOTE VIEWING FOR PARTICIPANTS (U) UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 WA Approved For Release 2003fftkiff P96-00788RO01 300070003-9 on INFORMATION ABOUT REMOTE VIEWING FOR PARTICIPANTS (U) (U) SRI International is carrying out a program to familiarize NO individuals with a human sensory capability that we call "remote viewing." Remote viewing (RV) is defined as the ability of a person to perceive, by an intellectual process, remote physical locations blocked from ordinary perception by distance or shielding. This includes the ability to view remote geographical locations, even at intercontinental distances, given only geographical coordinates or a known person on whom to focus. (U) It appears that individuals inexperienced at RV nonetheless exhibit the faculty, indicating that such functioning may be a latent and widely distributed, though undeveloped, perceptual ability. (U) The purpose of this project is to familiarize you with the RV capability so that; (a) you will have an opportunity to exercise and develop your innate RV faculty, and (b) you will be in a position to utilize RV in the performance of your regular professional duties. (S) The general outline of RV procedure is given in paragraphs 7 and 8 of, "Proposed SRI International Applied Remote Viewing Protocol (S)," dated 23 March 1979. You as a remote viewer may perform one or more types of remote viewing as outlined in these paragraphs. Your remote viewing performance will be evaluated according to procedures described in para- graph 14 of the same document, at which time you will receive information about the targets and an assessment of how closely your RV images match target site characteristics. (U) The maximum frequency you may be asked to perform RV is two times per day, ten times per week. You may choose not to perform RV at SECRET Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 No Approved For Relea U NeIrA ftffirly 788ROO1300070003-9 Mi (U) a particular time without prejudice as to further participation in this program, and you may discontinue participation permanently at any time without prejudice. (U) There is no evidence of adverse effects, physical, psychological, or social, on the remote viewer due to participation in this activity. dw (U) With regard to confidentiality, individuals performing as remote viewers and interviewers in this program will not be identified without their prior consent, and they will be referred to in project records only by an alpha-numeric designator. Products of remote viewers and interviewers such as tapes, drawings, transcripts, rosters, or other materials which id might reveal the identity of the remote viewer will be coded to assure the protection of their identity. 2 UNCLASSIFIED Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01 300070003-9 Approved For Release 2003/09/10 : CIA-RDP96-00788ROO1300070003-9 -19 SECRET A I Im -4 Approved For Release 20OW- 190 FIM-,m8rP96-00788 ROO 1300070003-9 01 @q